The Representation of Coordinate Relations in Lexical Semantic Memory
Two experiments examined the size of the typicality effect for true items in a category verification task as a function of the type of false item used. In Experiment 1, compared to the case where false items paired unrelated concepts ("carrot-vehicle"), the typicality effect was much large...
Saved in:
Published in | Frontiers in psychology Vol. 11; p. 98 |
---|---|
Main Author | |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Switzerland
Frontiers Media S.A
11.02.2020
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1664-1078 1664-1078 |
DOI | 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00098 |
Cover
Abstract | Two experiments examined the size of the typicality effect for true items in a category verification task as a function of the type of false item used. In Experiment 1, compared to the case where false items paired unrelated concepts ("carrot-vehicle"), the typicality effect was much larger when false items paired an exemplar with a category coordinate to its proper category ("carrot-fruit"). In contrast, when false items paired coordinate concepts ("carrot-pea") or reversed the ordering of subject and predicate terms ("All vegetables are carrots"), the typicality effect did not change in size. Further, the time to verify true sentences did not increase monotonically with the semantic similarity of the two terms used in false sentences. Experiment 2 showed that the pattern of results for coordinate items reflected semantic processing, not simply task difficulty. A combined analysis examined data across multiple experiments, increasing the power of the statistical analysis. The size of the typicality effect when coordinate false items were used was again the same as when false items paired unrelated concepts. The most straightforward explanation of this pattern of results seems to be in terms of a
model of lexical semantic memory, in which labeled links are used to indicate the semantic relation that exists between pairs of words. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Two experiments examined the size of the typicality effect for true items in a category verification task as a function of the type of false item used. In Experiment 1, compared to the case where false items paired unrelated concepts ("carrot-vehicle"), the typicality effect was much larger when false items paired an exemplar with a category coordinate to its proper category ("carrot-fruit"). In contrast, when false items paired coordinate concepts ("carrot-pea") or reversed the ordering of subject and predicate terms ("All vegetables are carrots"), the typicality effect did not change in size. Further, the time to verify true sentences did not increase monotonically with the semantic similarity of the two terms used in false sentences. Experiment 2 showed that the pattern of results for coordinate items reflected semantic processing, not simply task difficulty. A combined analysis examined data across multiple experiments, increasing the power of the statistical analysis. The size of the typicality effect when coordinate false items were used was again the same as when false items paired unrelated concepts. The most straightforward explanation of this pattern of results seems to be in terms of a sparse semantic network model of lexical semantic memory, in which labeled links are used to indicate the semantic relation that exists between pairs of words.Two experiments examined the size of the typicality effect for true items in a category verification task as a function of the type of false item used. In Experiment 1, compared to the case where false items paired unrelated concepts ("carrot-vehicle"), the typicality effect was much larger when false items paired an exemplar with a category coordinate to its proper category ("carrot-fruit"). In contrast, when false items paired coordinate concepts ("carrot-pea") or reversed the ordering of subject and predicate terms ("All vegetables are carrots"), the typicality effect did not change in size. Further, the time to verify true sentences did not increase monotonically with the semantic similarity of the two terms used in false sentences. Experiment 2 showed that the pattern of results for coordinate items reflected semantic processing, not simply task difficulty. A combined analysis examined data across multiple experiments, increasing the power of the statistical analysis. The size of the typicality effect when coordinate false items were used was again the same as when false items paired unrelated concepts. The most straightforward explanation of this pattern of results seems to be in terms of a sparse semantic network model of lexical semantic memory, in which labeled links are used to indicate the semantic relation that exists between pairs of words. Two experiments examined the size of the typicality effect for true items in a category verification task as a function of the type of false item used. In Experiment 1, compared to the case where false items paired unrelated concepts (“carrot–vehicle”), the typicality effect was much larger when false items paired an exemplar with a category coordinate to its proper category (“carrot–fruit”). In contrast, when false items paired coordinate concepts (“carrot–pea”) or reversed the ordering of subject and predicate terms (“All vegetables are carrots”), the typicality effect did not change in size. Further, the time to verify true sentences did not increase monotonically with the semantic similarity of the two terms used in false sentences. Experiment 2 showed that the pattern of results for coordinate items reflected semantic processing, not simply task difficulty. A combined analysis examined data across multiple experiments, increasing the power of the statistical analysis. The size of the typicality effect when coordinate false items were used was again the same as when false items paired unrelated concepts. The most straightforward explanation of this pattern of results seems to be in terms of a sparse semantic network model of lexical semantic memory, in which labeled links are used to indicate the semantic relation that exists between pairs of words. Two experiments examined the size of the typicality effect for true items in a category verification task as a function of the type of false item used. In Experiment 1, compared to the case where false items paired unrelated concepts (“carrot–vehicle”), the typicality effect was much larger when false items paired an exemplar with a category coordinate to its proper category (“carrot–fruit”). In contrast, when false items paired coordinate concepts (“carrot–pea”) or reversed the ordering of subject and predicate terms (“All vegetables are carrots”), the typicality effect did not change in size. Further, the time to verify true sentences did not increase monotonically with the semantic similarity of the two terms used in false sentences. Experiment 2 showed that the pattern of results for coordinate items reflected semantic processing, not simply task difficulty. A combined analysis examined data across multiple experiments, increasing the power of the statistical analysis. The size of the typicality effect when coordinate false items were used was again the same as when false items paired unrelated concepts. The most straightforward explanation of this pattern of results seems to be in terms of a sparse semantic network model of lexical semantic memory, in which labeled links are used to indicate the semantic relation that exists between pairs of words. Two experiments examined the size of the typicality effect for true items in a category verification task as a function of the type of false item used. In Experiment 1, compared to the case where false items paired unrelated concepts ("carrot-vehicle"), the typicality effect was much larger when false items paired an exemplar with a category coordinate to its proper category ("carrot-fruit"). In contrast, when false items paired coordinate concepts ("carrot-pea") or reversed the ordering of subject and predicate terms ("All vegetables are carrots"), the typicality effect did not change in size. Further, the time to verify true sentences did not increase monotonically with the semantic similarity of the two terms used in false sentences. Experiment 2 showed that the pattern of results for coordinate items reflected semantic processing, not simply task difficulty. A combined analysis examined data across multiple experiments, increasing the power of the statistical analysis. The size of the typicality effect when coordinate false items were used was again the same as when false items paired unrelated concepts. The most straightforward explanation of this pattern of results seems to be in terms of a model of lexical semantic memory, in which labeled links are used to indicate the semantic relation that exists between pairs of words. |
Author | Gruenenfelder, Thomas M. |
AuthorAffiliation | Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University Bloomington , Bloomington, IN , United States |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University Bloomington , Bloomington, IN , United States |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Thomas M. surname: Gruenenfelder fullname: Gruenenfelder, Thomas M. |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32116912$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNpVkU1vEzEQhi1URD_onRPaI5cEf6_3goSiQisFIUHu1tiZTV3t2sHeVOTf4ySlak-2Zh4_M9Z7Sc5iikjIB0bnQpjuc78t-82cU07nlNLOvCEXTGs5Y7Q1Zy_u5-S6lIeKUFlZyt-Rc8EZ0x3jF-RmdY_NL9xmLBgnmEKKTeqbRUp5HSJMh-ZwLJcmxGaJf4OHofmNI8Qp-OYHjinv35O3PQwFr5_OK7L6drNa3M6WP7_fLb4uZ14yNc0Md4iUM4ncKYboe9Y71J0D6lC6Hj0IKbWSHYierQ1wR41T2mvhvGLiitydtOsED3abwwh5bxMEeyykvLGQ61YDWiaMotxAfSVlx8A45FBHC9Vx5VtVXV9Oru3Ojbj29fcZhlfS150Y7u0mPdqWci10VwWfngQ5_dlhmewYisdhgIhpVyyvkDGt5m1FP76c9TzkfwwVoCfA51RKxv4ZYdQewrbHsO0hbHsMW_wDF26dPQ |
Cites_doi | 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01106.x 10.1016/s0022-5371(73)80056-8 10.3758/bf03204766 10.1111/1467-9280.00067 10.3758/bf03211304 10.1111/cogs.12299 10.3758/bf03333670 10.1006/jmla.1999.2650 10.1037/h0029436 10.1037/h0036351 10.18653/v1/P16-2074 10.3758/bf03202461 10.1080/09296170500172403 10.1016/0001-6918(79)90026-x 10.1037/13493-002 10.3758/bf03192726 10.1037/h0033172 10.1111/j.1469-8137.1912.tb05611.x 10.3758/bf03196311 10.1016/s0022-5371(81)90193-6 10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.192 10.1037/xlm0000391 10.3758/bf03212999 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01068.x 10.1037/0096-1523.7.3.688 10.1016/j.jml.2012.07.005 10.1037/0278-7393.32.4.643 10.1037/0278-7393.21.1.3 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01467.x 10.3758/pbr.15.3.598 10.1037/0096-3445.126.2.99 10.1016/j.jml.2006.04.001 10.1016/0001-6918(69)90055-9 10.1037/0033-295x.104.2.211 10.1098/rstb.2019.0305 10.1037/h0022853 10.1016/0010-0277(74)90002-x 10.1016/0010-0285(79)90002-1 10.18653/v1/P16-1226 10.3758/s13423-018-1453-6 10.1037/h0087421 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.1 10.1016/s0022-5371(75)80066-1 10.1037/0278-7393.31.5.905 10.7860/JCDR/2014/7740.4157 10.1037/0278-7393.12.4.587 10.1016/0022-5371(76)90053-0 10.1016/s0022-5371(82)90736-8 10.1037/0033-295x.85.2.59 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225 10.1080/09540099408915732 10.3758/bf03198427 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.01.001 10.3758/bf03200643 10.1016/s0022-5371(77)80043-1 10.1016/0022-2496(75)90053-X 10.1016/s0022-5371(69)80069-1 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright © 2020 Gruenenfelder. Copyright © 2020 Gruenenfelder. 2020 Gruenenfelder |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright © 2020 Gruenenfelder. – notice: Copyright © 2020 Gruenenfelder. 2020 Gruenenfelder |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION NPM 7X8 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00098 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic PubMed |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1664-1078 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_1385028a5134491a8be2a02135925c75 PMC7026369 32116912 10_3389_fpsyg_2020_00098 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | 53G 5VS 9T4 AAFWJ AAKDD AAYXX ABIVO ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACXDI ADBBV ADRAZ AEGXH AFPKN AIAGR ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS BAWUL BCNDV CITATION DIK EBS EJD EMOBN F5P GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 HYE KQ8 M48 M~E O5R O5S OK1 P2P PGMZT RNS RPM IAO ICO IEA IHR IHW IPNFZ IPY NPM RIG 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c415t-82bee0214e2b51eecf1fbe69ba0be4bfeca3446549a3f1d8a2b08b56c63bc513 |
IEDL.DBID | M48 |
ISSN | 1664-1078 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:24:56 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 18:20:57 EDT 2025 Fri Sep 05 00:40:48 EDT 2025 Thu Jan 02 22:59:13 EST 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:20:29 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Keywords | associative networks semantic relations coordinate relations lexical semantic memory semantic networks co-hyponyms hyponyms semantic memory |
Language | English |
License | Copyright © 2020 Gruenenfelder. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c415t-82bee0214e2b51eecf1fbe69ba0be4bfeca3446549a3f1d8a2b08b56c63bc513 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 This article was submitted to Cognition, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology Reviewed by: Fritz Gunther, University of Milano Bicocca, Italy; Gregory L. Murphy, New York University, United States Edited by: Rolf Ulrich, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany |
OpenAccessLink | http://journals.scholarsportal.info/openUrl.xqy?doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00098 |
PMID | 32116912 |
PQID | 2369887627 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_1385028a5134491a8be2a02135925c75 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_7026369 proquest_miscellaneous_2369887627 pubmed_primary_32116912 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2020_00098 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2020-02-11 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2020-02-11 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 02 year: 2020 text: 2020-02-11 day: 11 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | Switzerland |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Switzerland |
PublicationTitle | Frontiers in psychology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Front Psychol |
PublicationYear | 2020 |
Publisher | Frontiers Media S.A |
Publisher_xml | – name: Frontiers Media S.A |
References | Chaffin (B4) 1981; 17 Collins (B7) 1969; 8 Mikolov (B41) 2013; 26 Lamming (B26) 1979; 43 Rabbitt (B53) 1966; 71 Baroni (B1) 2012 Ratcliff (B55) 1978; 85 Hino (B17) 2006; 55 Jaccard (B20) 1912; 11 Masson (B36) 1995; 21 Weeds (B66) 2014 Jones (B22) 2007; 114 Smith (B64); 81 Church (B6) 1990; 16 King (B25) 1976; 15 Jescheniak (B21) 2005; 31 Gellatly (B10) 1977; 16 Holyoak (B18) 1975; 14 Landauer (B27) 1997; 104 Raaijmakers (B52) 1999; 41 Hampton (B16) 1997; 25 Murphy (B43) 2012; 67 Chaffin (B5) 1984; 12 Pexman (B49) 2003; 31 Lorch (B31) 1982; 21 McCloskey (B37) 1979; 11 Glass (B11) 1975; 3 Nguyen (B46) 2016 Rips (B57) 1973; 12 McRae (B39) 1997; 126 Pexman (B50) 2002; 9 Gruenenfelder (B15) 2016; 40 McRae (B38) 2005; 37 Hutchison (B19) 2003; 10 Link (B29) 1975; 12 McRae (B40) 2012 Cree (B8) 2006; 32 Gruenenfelder (B12) 1984; 16 Wettler (B67) 2005; 12 Nguyen (B45) Smith (B63) Rouder (B60) 2009; 16 Gellatly (B9) 1975; 66 Burgess (B3) 1998; 30 Rosch (B59) 1975; 104 Maki (B35) 2008; 15 Roller (B58) 2014 Shwartz (B61) 2016 Raaijmakers (B51) 2003; 57 Lucas (B33) 2000; 7 Louwerse (B32) 2011; 3 Sternberg (B65) 1969; 30 Baroni (B2) 2010; 34 Smith (B62) 1978 Olson (B47) 1970; 77 Perfetti (B48) 1972; 78 Lenci (B28) 2012 Nguyen (B44) Rabbitt (B54) 1977; 29 Moss (B42) 1994; 6 Lund (B34) 1996; 28 Gruenenfelder (B13) 1986; 12 Kiger (B24) 1981; 7 Kenett (B23) 2017; 43 Gruenenfelder (B14) 2020 Lorch (B30) 1981; 20 Ratcliff (B56) 1998; 9 |
References_xml | – volume: 3 start-page: 273 year: 2011 ident: B32 article-title: Symbol interdependency in symbolic and embodied cognition. publication-title: Top. Cogn. Sci. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01106.x – volume: 12 start-page: 1 year: 1973 ident: B57 article-title: Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations. publication-title: J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5371(73)80056-8 – volume: 28 start-page: 203 year: 1996 ident: B34 article-title: Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence. publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. doi: 10.3758/bf03204766 – volume: 9 start-page: 347 year: 1998 ident: B56 article-title: Modeling response times for two-choice decisions. publication-title: Psychol. Sci. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00067 – volume: 25 start-page: 625 year: 1997 ident: B16 article-title: Associative and similarity-based process in categorization decisions. publication-title: Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.3758/bf03211304 – ident: B45 article-title: Distinguishing antonyms and synonyms in a pattern-based neural network publication-title: Proceedings of the 15th Conference of the European Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics – volume: 40 start-page: 1460 year: 2016 ident: B15 article-title: Graph-theoretic properties of networks based on word association norms: implications for models of lexical semantic memory. publication-title: Cogn. Sci. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12299 – start-page: 23 year: 2012 ident: B1 article-title: Entailment above the word level in distributional semantics publication-title: Proceedings of the 13th Conference of the European Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics – volume: 17 start-page: 69 year: 1981 ident: B4 article-title: Comprehension of semantic relationships and the generality of categorization models. publication-title: Bull. Psychon. Soc. doi: 10.3758/bf03333670 – volume: 41 start-page: 416 year: 1999 ident: B52 article-title: How to deal with the “the language-as-fixed-effect fallacy”: common misconceptions and alternative solutions. publication-title: J. Mem. Lang. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2650 – volume: 77 start-page: 257 year: 1970 ident: B47 article-title: Language and thought: aspects of a cognitive theory of semantics. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/h0029436 – start-page: 1025 year: 2014 ident: B58 article-title: Inclusive yet selective: supervised distributional hypernymy detection publication-title: Proceedings of COLING 2014, the 25th International Conference on Computational Linguistics: Technical Papers – volume: 81 start-page: 214 ident: B64 article-title: Structure and process in semantic memory: a featural model for semantic decisions. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/h0036351 – year: 2016 ident: B46 article-title: Integrating distributional lexical contrast into word embeddings for antonym-synonym distinction publication-title: Proceedings of the 54th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics doi: 10.18653/v1/P16-2074 – start-page: 2249 year: 2014 ident: B66 article-title: Learning to distinguish hypernyms and co-hyponyms publication-title: Proceedings of COLING 2014, the 25th International Conference on Computational Linguistics: Technical Papers – volume: 16 start-page: 351 year: 1984 ident: B12 article-title: Typicality ratings for 893 exemplars of 93 categories. publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. doi: 10.3758/bf03202461 – volume: 12 start-page: 111 year: 2005 ident: B67 article-title: Free word associations correspond to contiguities between words in text. publication-title: J. Quant. Linguist. doi: 10.1080/09296170500172403 – volume: 43 start-page: 199 year: 1979 ident: B26 article-title: Choice reaction performance following an error. publication-title: Acta Psychol. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(79)90026-x – start-page: 39 year: 2012 ident: B40 article-title: Semantic and associative relations: examining a tenuous dichotomy publication-title: The Adolescent Brain: Learning, Reasoning, and Decision Making doi: 10.1037/13493-002 – volume: 37 start-page: 547 year: 2005 ident: B38 article-title: Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and non-living things. publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. doi: 10.3758/bf03192726 – volume: 78 start-page: 241 year: 1972 ident: B48 article-title: Psychosemantics: some cognitive aspects of structural meaning. publication-title: Psychol. Bull. doi: 10.1037/h0033172 – volume: 11 start-page: 37 year: 1912 ident: B20 article-title: The distribution of the flora in the Alpine Zone. publication-title: New Phytol. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8137.1912.tb05611.x – volume: 9 start-page: 542 year: 2002 ident: B50 article-title: The impact of feedback semantics in visual word recognition: number of features effects in lexical decision and naming tasks. publication-title: Psychon. Bull. Rev. doi: 10.3758/bf03196311 – volume: 20 start-page: 593 year: 1981 ident: B30 article-title: Effects of relation strength and semantic overlap in retrieval and comparison processes during sentence verification. publication-title: J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5371(81)90193-6 – volume: 104 start-page: 192 year: 1975 ident: B59 article-title: Cognitive representations of semantic categories. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.192 – volume: 43 start-page: 1470 year: 2017 ident: B23 article-title: The semantic distance task: quantifying semantic distance with semantic network path length. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000391 – volume: 7 start-page: 618 year: 2000 ident: B33 article-title: Semantic priming without association: a meta-analytic review. publication-title: Psychon. Bull. Rev. doi: 10.3758/bf03212999 – volume: 34 start-page: 222 year: 2010 ident: B2 article-title: Strudel: a corpus-based semantic model based on properties and types. publication-title: Cogn. Sci. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01068.x – volume: 7 start-page: 688 year: 1981 ident: B24 article-title: Context effects in sentence verification. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.7.3.688 – volume: 67 start-page: 521 year: 2012 ident: B43 article-title: Semantic memory redux: an experimental test of hierarchical category representation. publication-title: J. Mem. Lang. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.07.005 – year: 2020 ident: B14 publication-title: Interference Can Affect Response Times to False Items in Category Verification. – volume: 32 start-page: 643 year: 2006 ident: B8 article-title: Distinctive features hold a privileged status in the computation of word meaning: implications for theories of semantic memory. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.4.643 – volume: 21 start-page: 3 year: 1995 ident: B36 article-title: A distributed memory model of semantic priming. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.21.1.3 – volume: 66 start-page: 311 year: 1975 ident: B9 article-title: The effects of negative relatedness upon word-picture and word-word comparisons and subsequent recall. publication-title: Br. J. Psychol. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1975.tb01467.x – volume: 15 start-page: 598 year: 2008 ident: B35 article-title: Latent structure in measures of associative, semantic, and thematic knowledge. publication-title: Psychon. Bull. Rev. doi: 10.3758/pbr.15.3.598 – volume: 126 start-page: 99 year: 1997 ident: B39 article-title: On the nature and scope of featural representation of word meaning. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.126.2.99 – year: 1978 ident: B62 article-title: Theories of semantic memory publication-title: Handbook of Learning and Cognitive Processes – volume: 55 start-page: 246 year: 2006 ident: B17 article-title: Ambiguity and relatedness effects in semantic tasks: are they due to semantic coding? publication-title: J. Mem. Lang. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.04.001 – volume: 30 start-page: 276 year: 1969 ident: B65 article-title: The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method. publication-title: Acta Psychol. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(69)90055-9 – volume: 104 start-page: 211 year: 1997 ident: B27 article-title: A solution to Plato’s problem: the latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.104.2.211 – volume: 26 start-page: 3111 year: 2013 ident: B41 article-title: Distributed representations of words and phrases and their compositionality. publication-title: Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0305 – volume: 71 start-page: 264 year: 1966 ident: B53 article-title: Errors and error-corrections in choice-response tasks. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/h0022853 – volume: 3 start-page: 313 year: 1975 ident: B11 article-title: Alternative conceptions of semantic theory. publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(74)90002-x – volume: 11 start-page: 1 year: 1979 ident: B37 article-title: Decision processes in verifying category membership statements: implications for models of semantic memory. publication-title: Cogn. Psychol. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(79)90002-1 – ident: B63 article-title: Semantic memory and psychological semantics publication-title: The Psychology of Learning and Motivation – year: 2016 ident: B61 article-title: Improving hypernymy detection with an integrated path-based and distributional method publication-title: Proceedings of the 54th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics doi: 10.18653/v1/P16-1226 – volume: 10 start-page: 785 year: 2003 ident: B19 article-title: Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review. publication-title: Psychon. Bull. Rev. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1453-6 – volume: 16 start-page: 22 year: 1990 ident: B6 article-title: Word association norms, mutual information, and lexicography. publication-title: Comput. Linguist. – volume: 57 start-page: 141 year: 2003 ident: B51 article-title: A further look at the “language-as-fixed-effect fallacy”. publication-title: Can. J. Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/h0087421 – volume: 114 start-page: 1 year: 2007 ident: B22 article-title: Representing word meaning and order information in a composite holographic lexicon. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.1 – ident: B44 article-title: Hierarchical embeddings for hypernymy detection and directionality publication-title: Proceedings of the 2017 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing – volume: 14 start-page: 215 year: 1975 ident: B18 article-title: The role of contradictions and counterexamples in the rejection of false sentences. publication-title: J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5371(75)80066-1 – volume: 31 start-page: 905 year: 2005 ident: B21 article-title: Context effects on lexical choice and lexical activation. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.5.905 – volume: 29 start-page: 727 year: 1977 ident: B54 article-title: What does a man do after he makes an error? An analysis of response programming. publication-title: Q. J. Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2014/7740.4157 – start-page: 75 year: 2012 ident: B28 article-title: Identifying hypernyms in distributional semantic spaces publication-title: Proceedings of the First Joint Conference on Lexical and Computational Semantics – volume: 12 start-page: 587 year: 1986 ident: B13 article-title: Relational similarity and context effects in category verification. publication-title: J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.12.4.587 – volume: 15 start-page: 587 year: 1976 ident: B25 article-title: Long-term memory search: an intersecting activation process. publication-title: J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. doi: 10.1016/0022-5371(76)90053-0 – volume: 21 start-page: 468 year: 1982 ident: B31 article-title: Priming and search processes in semantic memory: a test of three models of semantic activation. publication-title: J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5371(82)90736-8 – volume: 85 start-page: 59 year: 1978 ident: B55 article-title: A theory of memory retrieval. publication-title: Psychol. Rev. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.85.2.59 – volume: 16 start-page: 225 year: 2009 ident: B60 article-title: Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. publication-title: Psychon. Bull. Rev. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225 – volume: 6 start-page: 413 year: 1994 ident: B42 article-title: A distributed memory model of the associative boost in semantic priming. publication-title: Conn. Sci. doi: 10.1080/09540099408915732 – volume: 12 start-page: 134 year: 1984 ident: B5 article-title: The similarity and diversity of semantic relations. publication-title: Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.3758/bf03198427 – volume: 31 start-page: 842 year: 2003 ident: B49 article-title: Number-of-features effects and semantic processing. publication-title: Mem. Cogn. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.01.001 – volume: 30 start-page: 188 year: 1998 ident: B3 article-title: From simple associations to the building blocks of language: modeling meaning in memory with the HAL model. publication-title: Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. doi: 10.3758/bf03200643 – volume: 16 start-page: 505 year: 1977 ident: B10 article-title: Intercategory distance and categorization times: effects of negative-probe relatedness. publication-title: J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5371(77)80043-1 – volume: 12 start-page: 114 year: 1975 ident: B29 article-title: The relative judgment theory of two choice reaction time. publication-title: J. Math. Psychol. doi: 10.1016/0022-2496(75)90053-X – volume: 8 start-page: 240 year: 1969 ident: B7 article-title: Retrieval time from semantic memory. publication-title: J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5371(69)80069-1 |
SSID | ssj0000402002 |
Score | 2.227981 |
Snippet | Two experiments examined the size of the typicality effect for true items in a category verification task as a function of the type of false item used. In... |
SourceID | doaj pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database |
StartPage | 98 |
SubjectTerms | associative networks co-hyponyms coordinate relations lexical semantic memory Psychology semantic memory semantic networks |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1Na9wwEBUhp1xCv-t-oUIuPZhYkmVLxzYkhJDk0KaQm9DIo2QP9YZmA91_3xl5s-yWQi-92sYW7wnNPPT8JMQBlbScjNH1oEmutohtDb3tagPWZSqQTYu8o3tx2Z1-b8-u7fXGUV_sCZvigSfgDpVxlmpgtMq0rVfRAepIhclYr23qS3pp45sNMVXWYJZFbN3hfUlSYf4w390vb0gParZyNd5t1aES1_-3HvNPq-RG7Tl5IvZXTaP8PA32qdjB8ZnYW69dy-fimOiWX4urdfUz0SjnWR7NSVvORuon5dr1JmejPMdfTI78hj8I2VmSF2y4Xb4QVyfHV0en9eqEhDpR4V3UTgMip56hBqsQU1YZsPMQG8AWMqZoSmKajyarwUUNjQPbpc5AIkRfit1xPuJrIQdqYww1J10coM3WOQBvSCulXsFgtK3Ep0e4wt2UgxFIPzC0oUAbGNpQoK3EF8Zz_RwnWJcLxGtY8Rr-xWslPj6yEWjG8zZGHHH-cB-06bzjRbyvxKuJnfWnDOnZzitdiX6Lt62xbN8ZZ7clVbsnNUpvfvM_Bv9W7DEc7O5W6p3YXfx8wPfUvCzgQ5mnvwE7jurw priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals |
Title | The Representation of Coordinate Relations in Lexical Semantic Memory |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/32116912 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2369887627 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC7026369 https://doaj.org/article/1385028a5134491a8be2a02135925c75 |
Volume | 11 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV1Lb9QwELagXHpBvFkelZG4cAisn3EOCEHVqkKUA7RSb5btjNuVICm7W6n775lx0oVFe-CaRLbzje2ZT_N5zNhrdGk5KSWrViJd1QC6irWxlYrGZXSQUw2U0T3-ao9O9eczc_bnePQI4GIrtaP7pE7nP95e_1p9wAX_nhgnNvcuXy5W50j1JKm0po27ze6UbBEJ-cZgv-zLRJUGEaK1Gvef2g15y62NbPipUs5_Wwz6r5TyL990eI_dHYNK_nGYBffZLegesN313rZ6yA5wOvBvRfU6HjbqeJ_5fo_cc9ZhvMnXqjg-6_gXuCbj8e_wE5GfJX5MgtzVI3ZyeHCyf1SNNyhUCR3zsnIyAlBVNJDRCICURY5gmximEXTMkIIqFdWaoLJoXZBx6qKxyaqYjFCP2U7Xd_CU8RbDHIXBiw1t1Nk4F2OjkEulWsRWSTNhb27g8pdDnQyP_IKg9QVaT9D6Au2EfSI8199RhevyoJ-f-3HBeKGcwdgn4Ci0bkRwEWTAX1GmkSbV2OGrG2t4XBGU5ggd9FcLL5VtHG3y9YQ9Gayz7koh37WNkBNWb9htYyybb7rZRam6XSNbxZaf_Ue_z9ku_S2Ju4V4wXaW8yt4ibHLMu4Vzr9XJuZvxqjtTw |
linkProvider | Scholars Portal |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+Representation+of+Coordinate+Relations+in+Lexical+Semantic+Memory&rft.jtitle=Frontiers+in+psychology&rft.au=Gruenenfelder%2C+Thomas+M&rft.date=2020-02-11&rft.issn=1664-1078&rft.eissn=1664-1078&rft.volume=11&rft.spage=98&rft_id=info:doi/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2020.00098&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |