Model of analysis in two halves for semantic fluency tasks
Semantic verbal fluency (SVF) tasks involve the activation of language, semantic memory, working memory and the executive functions. The second half of the test increases the demands on working memory and search for lexical items, which can make it more difficult than the first half. To conduct a pi...
Saved in:
Published in | Revista de neurologiá Vol. 44; no. 9; p. 531 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | Spanish |
Published |
Spain
01.05.2007
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0210-0010 |
DOI | 10.33588/rn.4409.2005690 |
Cover
Abstract | Semantic verbal fluency (SVF) tasks involve the activation of language, semantic memory, working memory and the executive functions. The second half of the test increases the demands on working memory and search for lexical items, which can make it more difficult than the first half.
To conduct a pilot study in order to standardise the two halves of the SVF test ('animals' category) in adults over 50 years of age.
Two verbal fluency tasks (semantic and phonological) and the Spanish version of the Mini-Mental State Examination with working memory were applied in 122 controls (range: 50-89 years), who were distributed according to age and schooling.
There is a significant difference between the two halves of the test adjusted for age and schooling (p < 0.0001). Scoring in the second half showed a higher correlation with age, schooling and working memory than the first half. Performance in the two halves was standardised for three age groups (50-59, 60-79 and 80-89 years) and two levels of schooling (high and low).
The number of names in the second half of the SVF test was lower than in the first half in all the groups, which supports the hypothesis of a greater cognitive demand, probably on working memory and the search for lexical items in semantic memory. Having normative values available for the two halves of the SVF test allows simultaneous and independent interpretation of the performance at two levels of intra-task difficulty. This model of analysis complements the traditional assessment and can easily be applied in day-to-day clinical practice. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Semantic verbal fluency (SVF) tasks involve the activation of language, semantic memory, working memory and the executive functions. The second half of the test increases the demands on working memory and search for lexical items, which can make it more difficult than the first half.INTRODUCTIONSemantic verbal fluency (SVF) tasks involve the activation of language, semantic memory, working memory and the executive functions. The second half of the test increases the demands on working memory and search for lexical items, which can make it more difficult than the first half.To conduct a pilot study in order to standardise the two halves of the SVF test ('animals' category) in adults over 50 years of age.AIMTo conduct a pilot study in order to standardise the two halves of the SVF test ('animals' category) in adults over 50 years of age.Two verbal fluency tasks (semantic and phonological) and the Spanish version of the Mini-Mental State Examination with working memory were applied in 122 controls (range: 50-89 years), who were distributed according to age and schooling.SUBJECTS AND METHODSTwo verbal fluency tasks (semantic and phonological) and the Spanish version of the Mini-Mental State Examination with working memory were applied in 122 controls (range: 50-89 years), who were distributed according to age and schooling.There is a significant difference between the two halves of the test adjusted for age and schooling (p < 0.0001). Scoring in the second half showed a higher correlation with age, schooling and working memory than the first half. Performance in the two halves was standardised for three age groups (50-59, 60-79 and 80-89 years) and two levels of schooling (high and low).RESULTSThere is a significant difference between the two halves of the test adjusted for age and schooling (p < 0.0001). Scoring in the second half showed a higher correlation with age, schooling and working memory than the first half. Performance in the two halves was standardised for three age groups (50-59, 60-79 and 80-89 years) and two levels of schooling (high and low).The number of names in the second half of the SVF test was lower than in the first half in all the groups, which supports the hypothesis of a greater cognitive demand, probably on working memory and the search for lexical items in semantic memory. Having normative values available for the two halves of the SVF test allows simultaneous and independent interpretation of the performance at two levels of intra-task difficulty. This model of analysis complements the traditional assessment and can easily be applied in day-to-day clinical practice.CONCLUSIONSThe number of names in the second half of the SVF test was lower than in the first half in all the groups, which supports the hypothesis of a greater cognitive demand, probably on working memory and the search for lexical items in semantic memory. Having normative values available for the two halves of the SVF test allows simultaneous and independent interpretation of the performance at two levels of intra-task difficulty. This model of analysis complements the traditional assessment and can easily be applied in day-to-day clinical practice. Semantic verbal fluency (SVF) tasks involve the activation of language, semantic memory, working memory and the executive functions. The second half of the test increases the demands on working memory and search for lexical items, which can make it more difficult than the first half. To conduct a pilot study in order to standardise the two halves of the SVF test ('animals' category) in adults over 50 years of age. Two verbal fluency tasks (semantic and phonological) and the Spanish version of the Mini-Mental State Examination with working memory were applied in 122 controls (range: 50-89 years), who were distributed according to age and schooling. There is a significant difference between the two halves of the test adjusted for age and schooling (p < 0.0001). Scoring in the second half showed a higher correlation with age, schooling and working memory than the first half. Performance in the two halves was standardised for three age groups (50-59, 60-79 and 80-89 years) and two levels of schooling (high and low). The number of names in the second half of the SVF test was lower than in the first half in all the groups, which supports the hypothesis of a greater cognitive demand, probably on working memory and the search for lexical items in semantic memory. Having normative values available for the two halves of the SVF test allows simultaneous and independent interpretation of the performance at two levels of intra-task difficulty. This model of analysis complements the traditional assessment and can easily be applied in day-to-day clinical practice. |
Author | Santos-Lasaosa, S Fernández-Turrado, T Mostacero-Miguel, E Larrodé-Pellicer, P Pascual-Millán, L F Fernández-Arín, E |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: T surname: Fernández-Turrado fullname: Fernández-Turrado, T email: tfertur@unizar.es organization: Unidad FCS y Demencias, Hospital Clínico Universitario Lozano Blesa, 50009 Zaragoza, España. tfertur@unizar.es – sequence: 2 givenname: L F surname: Pascual-Millán fullname: Pascual-Millán, L F – sequence: 3 givenname: E surname: Fernández-Arín fullname: Fernández-Arín, E – sequence: 4 givenname: P surname: Larrodé-Pellicer fullname: Larrodé-Pellicer, P – sequence: 5 givenname: S surname: Santos-Lasaosa fullname: Santos-Lasaosa, S – sequence: 6 givenname: E surname: Mostacero-Miguel fullname: Mostacero-Miguel, E |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17492611$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNo1jztPwzAUhT0U0QfsTMgTW8p17Ni5bKjiJRWxwBw5ybUIOHaJE1D_PZUo05GOPn06Z8lmIQZi7ELAWsqiLK-HsFYKcJ0DFBphxhaQC8gABMzZMqUPACUVwimbC6Mw10Is2M1zbMnz6LgN1u9Tl3gX-PgT-bv135S4iwNP1Nswdg13fqLQ7Plo02c6YyfO-kTnx1yxt_u7181jtn15eNrcbrOdkDhmqE2tJWht8tao_DBAKeeMLElgra0uGkRHCl2hTXEoSINTSLpROWIpQa7Y1Z93N8SvidJY9V1qyHsbKE6pMqBKJUAfwMsjONU9tdVu6Ho77Kv_t_IXoxRVBA |
ContentType | Journal Article |
DBID | CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 |
DOI | 10.33588/rn.4409.2005690 |
DatabaseName | Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
DocumentTitleAlternate | Modelo de análisis en dos mitades para tareas de fluidez semántica |
ExternalDocumentID | 17492611 |
Genre | English Abstract Journal Article |
GeographicLocations | Spain |
GeographicLocations_xml | – name: Spain |
GroupedDBID | 123 2WC 53G ADBBV AENEX ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS BAWUL CGR CUY CVF DIK ECM EIF F5P NPM OK1 Z7D ~OR 7X8 PGMZT |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-p139t-967b6306672d74249044ff738e19b6a65c99fe49f5675b6ae60f49e6c42998303 |
ISSN | 0210-0010 |
IngestDate | Fri Jul 11 16:47:13 EDT 2025 Sat Sep 28 07:46:51 EDT 2024 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 9 |
Language | Spanish |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-p139t-967b6306672d74249044ff738e19b6a65c99fe49f5675b6ae60f49e6c42998303 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-2 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-1 content type line 23 |
PMID | 17492611 |
PQID | 70484106 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_70484106 pubmed_primary_17492611 |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2007 May 1-15 20070501 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2007-05-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 05 year: 2007 text: 2007 May 1-15 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2000 |
PublicationPlace | Spain |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Spain |
PublicationTitle | Revista de neurologiá |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Rev Neurol |
PublicationYear | 2007 |
SSID | ssj0043490 |
Score | 1.7898507 |
Snippet | Semantic verbal fluency (SVF) tasks involve the activation of language, semantic memory, working memory and the executive functions. The second half of the... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database |
StartPage | 531 |
SubjectTerms | Adult Age Factors Aged Aged, 80 and over Animals Cognition Disorders - diagnosis Cognition Disorders - physiopathology Educational Status Female Humans Language Tests - standards Male Memory - physiology Middle Aged Neuropsychological Tests Pilot Projects Semantics Spain Statistics as Topic Verbal Behavior - physiology |
Title | Model of analysis in two halves for semantic fluency tasks |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17492611 https://www.proquest.com/docview/70484106 |
Volume | 44 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
journalDatabaseRights | – providerCode: PRVBFR databaseName: Free Medical Journals issn: 0210-0010 databaseCode: DIK dateStart: 19980101 customDbUrl: isFulltext: true dateEnd: 99991231 titleUrlDefault: http://www.freemedicaljournals.com omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0043490 providerName: Flying Publisher |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Jb9QwFLaGcuGCQGxtWXxAXEYuycR24t4q2qrAUOaQkeYWxYktVdBkOpkREr--79lOZxGI5RJFVpaZvE_P73srIW-1jupa25ilI5sxLuOMlbxSTICw06xKZMmxwPnLpbyY8k8zMRsM7EbW0mqpj6qfv6wr-R-pwhrIFatk_0Gydw-FBTgH-cIRJAzHv5IxDjJztmTZtxbBrMUfLU5IwW6ymELYmWv4eFfV0A8jAWOz7L51mzYphgfARhzWZui6W6I2dPHzO2mgs9mvoMeZ5avFoqzbrRTrCWa0lt8Z1haGSx3lX2cO7zzkxMfoT5utcogx9oSsffSeTbBZaOUxNdlyT6TrZMCgxUYYbY9C7mpQub7lY4CW2tCfwm8Ju3o9SUSGxQqL5ogDIXWuMOnHjG630L78WpxPx-MiP5vl7-Y3DKeLYRQ-jFq5R-6PUilx0sXpx8_9js0THnxx4af6cLZ76fudV_6eiDiDJH9EHgYmQU88LB6TgemekGMHCdpa2kOCXjUUIEE9JChAgvaQoAES1EHiKZmen-UfLlgYj8HmYLYvmZKplgkmKY_qlAONjji3Nk0yEystSykqpazhygoghbBgZGS5MrJCEyQD0-UZ2WvaxrwgtBZaW4vcWVheaalLZaUWVQb8Cxi22Cdv-n9dgPrBmFLZmHbVFSnsADyO5D557j9GMfddUgqgugpujg_-eO8hebBGzkuyt1yszCsw9Zb6tZPTLaAGUIA |
linkProvider | Flying Publisher |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Model+of+analysis+in+two+halves+for+semantic+fluency+tasks&rft.jtitle=Revista+de+neurologi%C3%A1&rft.au=Fern%C3%A1ndez-Turrado%2C+T&rft.au=Pascual-Mill%C3%A1n%2C+L+F&rft.au=Fern%C3%A1ndez-Ar%C3%ADn%2C+E&rft.au=Larrod%C3%A9-Pellicer%2C+P&rft.date=2007-05-01&rft.issn=0210-0010&rft.volume=44&rft.issue=9&rft.spage=531&rft_id=info:doi/10.33588%2Frn.4409.2005690&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0210-0010&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0210-0010&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0210-0010&client=summon |