Modeling individual differences in response time and accuracy in numeracy
•A diffusion model is fit to accuracy and RT distributions for three numeracy tasks.•The model extracts measures of components of the decision-making process.•There were strong correlations in model parameters between memory and numeracy tasks.•Individual differences showed drift rate was related to...
Saved in:
Published in | Cognition Vol. 137; pp. 115 - 136 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Netherlands
Elsevier B.V
01.04.2015
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0010-0277 1873-7838 1873-7838 |
DOI | 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004 |
Cover
Abstract | •A diffusion model is fit to accuracy and RT distributions for three numeracy tasks.•The model extracts measures of components of the decision-making process.•There were strong correlations in model parameters between memory and numeracy tasks.•Individual differences showed drift rate was related to accuracy and boundary to RT.•The diffusion model reconciles the lack of correlation between accuracy and RT.
In the study of numeracy, some hypotheses have been based on response time (RT) as a dependent variable and some on accuracy, and considerable controversy has arisen about the presence or absence of correlations between RT and accuracy, between RT or accuracy and individual differences like IQ and math ability, and between various numeracy tasks. In this article, we show that an integration of the two dependent variables is required, which we accomplish with a theory-based model of decision making. We report data from four tasks: numerosity discrimination, number discrimination, memory for two-digit numbers, and memory for three-digit numbers. Accuracy correlated across tasks, as did RTs. However, the negative correlations that might be expected between RT and accuracy were not obtained; if a subject was accurate, it did not mean that they were fast (and vice versa). When the diffusion decision-making model was applied to the data (Ratcliff, 1978), we found significant correlations across the tasks between the quality of the numeracy information (drift rate) driving the decision process and between the speed/accuracy criterion settings, suggesting that similar numeracy skills and similar speed–accuracy settings are involved in the four tasks. In the model, accuracy is related to drift rate and RT is related to speed–accuracy criteria, but drift rate and criteria are not related to each other across subjects. This provides a theoretical basis for understanding why negative correlations were not obtained between accuracy and RT. We also manipulated criteria by instructing subjects to maximize either speed or accuracy, but still found correlations between the criteria settings between and within tasks, suggesting that the settings may represent an individual trait that can be modulated but not equated across subjects. Our results demonstrate that a decision-making model may provide a way to reconcile inconsistent and sometimes contradictory results in numeracy research. |
---|---|
AbstractList | In the study of numeracy, some hypotheses have been based on response time (RT) as a dependent variable and some on accuracy, and considerable controversy has arisen about the presence or absence of correlations between RT and accuracy, between RT or accuracy and individual differences like IQ and math ability, and between various numeracy tasks. In this article, we show that an integration of the two dependent variables is required, which we accomplish with a theory-based model of decision making. We report data from four tasks: numerosity discrimination, number discrimination, memory for two-digit numbers, and memory for three-digit numbers. Accuracy correlated across tasks, as did RTs. However, the negative correlations that might be expected between RT and accuracy were not obtained; if a subject was accurate, it did not mean that they were fast (and vice versa). When the diffusion decision-making model was applied to the data (
Ratcliff, 1978
), we found significant correlations across the tasks between the quality of the numeracy information (drift rate) driving the decision process and between the speed/ accuracy criterion settings, suggesting that similar numeracy skills and similar speed-accuracy settings are involved in the four tasks. In the model, accuracy is related to drift rate and RT is related to speed-accuracy criteria, but drift rate and criteria are not related to each other across subjects. This provides a theoretical basis for understanding why negative correlations were not obtained between accuracy and RT. We also manipulated criteria by instructing subjects to maximize either speed or accuracy, but still found correlations between the criteria settings between and within tasks, suggesting that the settings may represent an individual trait that can be modulated but not equated across subjects. Our results demonstrate that a decision-making model may provide a way to reconcile inconsistent and sometimes contradictory results in numeracy research. In the study of numeracy, some hypotheses have been based on response time (RT) as a dependent variable and some on accuracy, and considerable controversy has arisen about the presence or absence of correlations between RT and accuracy, between RT or accuracy and individual differences like IQ and math ability, and between various numeracy tasks. In this article, we show that an integration of the two dependent variables is required, which we accomplish with a theory-based model of decision making. We report data from four tasks: numerosity discrimination, number discrimination, memory for two-digit numbers, and memory for three-digit numbers. Accuracy correlated across tasks, as did RTs. However, the negative correlations that might be expected between RT and accuracy were not obtained; if a subject was accurate, it did not mean that they were fast (and vice versa). When the diffusion decision-making model was applied to the data (Ratcliff, 1978), we found significant correlations across the tasks between the quality of the numeracy information (drift rate) driving the decision process and between the speed/accuracy criterion settings, suggesting that similar numeracy skills and similar speed-accuracy settings are involved in the four tasks. In the model, accuracy is related to drift rate and RT is related to speed-accuracy criteria, but drift rate and criteria are not related to each other across subjects. This provides a theoretical basis for understanding why negative correlations were not obtained between accuracy and RT. We also manipulated criteria by instructing subjects to maximize either speed or accuracy, but still found correlations between the criteria settings between and within tasks, suggesting that the settings may represent an individual trait that can be modulated but not equated across subjects. Our results demonstrate that a decision-making model may provide a way to reconcile inconsistent and sometimes contradictory results in numeracy research. •A diffusion model is fit to accuracy and RT distributions for three numeracy tasks.•The model extracts measures of components of the decision-making process.•There were strong correlations in model parameters between memory and numeracy tasks.•Individual differences showed drift rate was related to accuracy and boundary to RT.•The diffusion model reconciles the lack of correlation between accuracy and RT. In the study of numeracy, some hypotheses have been based on response time (RT) as a dependent variable and some on accuracy, and considerable controversy has arisen about the presence or absence of correlations between RT and accuracy, between RT or accuracy and individual differences like IQ and math ability, and between various numeracy tasks. In this article, we show that an integration of the two dependent variables is required, which we accomplish with a theory-based model of decision making. We report data from four tasks: numerosity discrimination, number discrimination, memory for two-digit numbers, and memory for three-digit numbers. Accuracy correlated across tasks, as did RTs. However, the negative correlations that might be expected between RT and accuracy were not obtained; if a subject was accurate, it did not mean that they were fast (and vice versa). When the diffusion decision-making model was applied to the data (Ratcliff, 1978), we found significant correlations across the tasks between the quality of the numeracy information (drift rate) driving the decision process and between the speed/accuracy criterion settings, suggesting that similar numeracy skills and similar speed–accuracy settings are involved in the four tasks. In the model, accuracy is related to drift rate and RT is related to speed–accuracy criteria, but drift rate and criteria are not related to each other across subjects. This provides a theoretical basis for understanding why negative correlations were not obtained between accuracy and RT. We also manipulated criteria by instructing subjects to maximize either speed or accuracy, but still found correlations between the criteria settings between and within tasks, suggesting that the settings may represent an individual trait that can be modulated but not equated across subjects. Our results demonstrate that a decision-making model may provide a way to reconcile inconsistent and sometimes contradictory results in numeracy research. In the study of numeracy, some hypotheses have been based on response time (RT) as a dependent variable and some on accuracy, and considerable controversy has arisen about the presence or absence of correlations between RT and accuracy, between RT or accuracy and individual differences like IQ and math ability, and between various numeracy tasks. In this article, we show that an integration of the two dependent variables is required, which we accomplish with a theory-based model of decision making. We report data from four tasks: numerosity discrimination, number discrimination, memory for two-digit numbers, and memory for three-digit numbers. Accuracy correlated across tasks, as did RTs. However, the negative correlations that might be expected between RT and accuracy were not obtained; if a subject was accurate, it did not mean that they were fast (and vice versa). When the diffusion decision-making model was applied to the data (Ratcliff, 1978), we found significant correlations across the tasks between the quality of the numeracy information (drift rate) driving the decision process and between the speed/accuracy criterion settings, suggesting that similar numeracy skills and similar speed-accuracy settings are involved in the four tasks. In the model, accuracy is related to drift rate and RT is related to speed-accuracy criteria, but drift rate and criteria are not related to each other across subjects. This provides a theoretical basis for understanding why negative correlations were not obtained between accuracy and RT. We also manipulated criteria by instructing subjects to maximize either speed or accuracy, but still found correlations between the criteria settings between and within tasks, suggesting that the settings may represent an individual trait that can be modulated but not equated across subjects. Our results demonstrate that a decision-making model may provide a way to reconcile inconsistent and sometimes contradictory results in numeracy research.In the study of numeracy, some hypotheses have been based on response time (RT) as a dependent variable and some on accuracy, and considerable controversy has arisen about the presence or absence of correlations between RT and accuracy, between RT or accuracy and individual differences like IQ and math ability, and between various numeracy tasks. In this article, we show that an integration of the two dependent variables is required, which we accomplish with a theory-based model of decision making. We report data from four tasks: numerosity discrimination, number discrimination, memory for two-digit numbers, and memory for three-digit numbers. Accuracy correlated across tasks, as did RTs. However, the negative correlations that might be expected between RT and accuracy were not obtained; if a subject was accurate, it did not mean that they were fast (and vice versa). When the diffusion decision-making model was applied to the data (Ratcliff, 1978), we found significant correlations across the tasks between the quality of the numeracy information (drift rate) driving the decision process and between the speed/accuracy criterion settings, suggesting that similar numeracy skills and similar speed-accuracy settings are involved in the four tasks. In the model, accuracy is related to drift rate and RT is related to speed-accuracy criteria, but drift rate and criteria are not related to each other across subjects. This provides a theoretical basis for understanding why negative correlations were not obtained between accuracy and RT. We also manipulated criteria by instructing subjects to maximize either speed or accuracy, but still found correlations between the criteria settings between and within tasks, suggesting that the settings may represent an individual trait that can be modulated but not equated across subjects. Our results demonstrate that a decision-making model may provide a way to reconcile inconsistent and sometimes contradictory results in numeracy research. |
Author | Thompson, Clarissa A. McKoon, Gail Ratcliff, Roger |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Roger surname: Ratcliff fullname: Ratcliff, Roger email: ratcliff.22@osu.edu organization: The Ohio State University, United States – sequence: 2 givenname: Clarissa A. surname: Thompson fullname: Thompson, Clarissa A. organization: Kent State University, United States – sequence: 3 givenname: Gail surname: McKoon fullname: McKoon, Gail organization: The Ohio State University, United States |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25637690$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqNkUtv1DAUhS1URKeFvwBZsknwK76eBUhVxaNSERtYW459M3iU2IOdjNR_T6JpK2BTVrZ8zzn3yN8FOYspIiFvGG0YZerdvnFpF8MUUmw4ZbJhvKFUPiMbpkHUoIU-IxtKGa0pBzgnF6Xs6aLgoF-Qc94qAWpLN-Tma_I4hLirQvThGPxsh8qHvseM0WFZnquM5ZBiwWoKI1Y2-so6N2fr7tZpnEdc7y_J894OBV_dn5fkx6eP36-_1LffPt9cX93WrlVsqh06aLGXjlGw0AE423etlehVx1iL2lqusNe87QQqpjkFAVYrhkIp3YK4JB9OuYe5G9E7jFO2gznkMNp8Z5IN5u9JDD_NLh2NFK2Q2-0S8PY-IKdfM5bJjKE4HAYbMc3FMOCgAJT-D6lSVErJxSp9_Wetxz4PX70I3p8ELqdSMvbGhcmuBJeWYTCMmhWt2ZtHtGZFaxg3C7jFD__4H1Y87bw6OXHBcgyYTXFhpetDRjcZn8KTGb8BwYnE0Q |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2021_101148 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_020_01405_4 crossref_primary_10_3390_biology10090902 crossref_primary_10_3390_jintelligence4030007 crossref_primary_10_1162_netn_a_00205 crossref_primary_10_1080_15248372_2022_2063293 crossref_primary_10_3390_bs13080646 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2024_106181 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_589305 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2015_10_009 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2022_08_015 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2016_04_005 crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218211041953 crossref_primary_10_1177_20416695241259160 crossref_primary_10_5964_jnc_v3i1_79 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2020_104219 crossref_primary_10_1007_s41465_025_00319_3 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogpsych_2019_101259 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_concog_2019_102797 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_018_1083_7 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2018_05_005 crossref_primary_10_1007_s12144_018_9899_2 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0163076 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_019_01268_1 crossref_primary_10_1177_0963721415596228 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_017_0921_7 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13234 crossref_primary_10_1080_10888438_2017_1287188 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogpsych_2022_101516 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2015_01955 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuron_2016_09_018 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_021_01900_5 crossref_primary_10_1177_17470218231197518 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13414_024_02870_1 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_14198_7 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2019_100812 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2023_105827 crossref_primary_10_1017_S136672892400110X crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_020_01371_8 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00426_016_0770_5 crossref_primary_10_1080_13803395_2019_1662374 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2017_01615 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2020_100888 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2019_00441 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci13071062 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2016_01_007 crossref_primary_10_1080_02643294_2022_2114824 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhy270 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0195188 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogpsych_2016_10_002 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2016_07_001 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13428_019_01302_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lindif_2023_102318 |
Cites_doi | 10.3758/s13423-011-0079-8 10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.261 10.1080/17470218.2011.574710 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.001 10.1111/1467-9280.00067 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.10.002 10.1037/a0020074 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01608.x 10.1037/a0018022 10.1038/nature07246 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01683.x 10.3758/s13423-011-0189-3 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.016 10.3758/BF03194580 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.010 10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.323 10.3758/BF03196302 10.1177/0956797610378309 10.3758/BF03193087 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.261 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.01.003 10.3758/s13423-011-0175-9 10.1037/a0030775 10.1016/j.jmp.2010.01.004 10.1037/0033-295X.113.4.700 10.3758/BF03193973 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.01.006 10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01263-6 10.1037/0033-295X.85.2.59 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.09.001 10.3758/s13423-011-0154-1 10.1016/j.jml.2003.11.002 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.02.008 10.3389/fninf.2013.00014 10.3758/BF03196283 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.550 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.353 10.3758/BF03192967 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.10.004 10.1073/pnas.1200196109 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.02.002 10.3758/PBR.16.4.742 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01080.x 10.1037/0012-1649.30.2.163 10.1037/0882-7974.18.3.415 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.003 10.1027/1618-3169/a000218 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.12.002 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 10.1162/neco.2008.12-06-420 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2014 Elsevier B.V. Copyright © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 2014 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2014 Elsevier B.V. – notice: Copyright © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. – notice: 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 2014 |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 7TK 5PM |
DOI | 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic Neurosciences Abstracts PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic Neurosciences Abstracts |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology Mathematics |
EISSN | 1873-7838 |
EndPage | 136 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC4353499 25637690 10_1016_j_cognition_2014_12_004 S0010027714002844 |
Genre | Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: NIA NIH HHS grantid: R01-AG041176 – fundername: NIA NIH HHS grantid: R01 AG041176 |
GroupedDBID | --- --K --M --Z -DZ -~X .~1 0R~ 186 1B1 1RT 1~. 1~5 29F 3EH 4.4 41~ 457 4G. 53G 5GY 5VS 6J9 6PF 7-5 71M 8P~ 9JM 9JO AABNK AACTN AADFP AADPK AAEDT AAEDW AAFJI AAGJA AAGUQ AAIAV AAIKJ AAKOC AALRI AAOAW AAQFI AAWTL AAXLA AAXUO ABCQJ ABFNM ABFRF ABIVO ABJNI ABLJU ABMAC ABMMH ABOYX ABXDB ABYKQ ACDAQ ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACIUM ACKIV ACNCT ACPRK ACRLP ACXNI ADBBV ADEZE ADIYS AEBSH AEFWE AEKER AETEA AFFNX AFKWA AFTJW AFXIZ AFYLN AGHFR AGUBO AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AIEXJ AIKHN AITUG AJBFU AJOXV AKYCK ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AMFUW AMRAJ AOMHK ASPBG AVARZ AVWKF AXJTR AZFZN BKOJK BLXMC CS3 DU5 EBS EFJIC EFLBG EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 F5P FD6 FDB FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU FYGXN G-2 G-Q G8K GBLVA HF~ HMQ HMW HVGLF HZ~ IHE J1W K-O KOM LPU M2V M3V M41 MO0 MOBAO MVM N9A NHB O-L O9- OAUVE OHT OKEIE OZT P-8 P-9 P2P PC. PRBVW Q38 R2- RIG ROL RPZ SCC SDF SDG SDP SES SEW SNS SPCBC SPS SSB SSN SSO SSY SSZ T5K TN5 UBW UPT UQL WH7 WUQ XFK XIH XJT XKC XOL XPP YYP YZZ ZA5 ZKB ZMT ~G- AATTM AAXKI AAYWO AAYXX ABDPE ACVFH ADCNI ADMHG ADVLN AEIPS AEUPX AFJKZ AFPUW AGCQF AGRNS AIGII AIIUN AKBMS AKRWK AKYEP ANKPU APXCP BNPGV CITATION SSH CGR CUY CVF ECM EFKBS EIF NPM 7X8 ACLOT ~HD 7TK 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c561t-cec75ef4c107a7b77cafb5a4ed6b115e8aa26ef825b3e61820737a861e3668573 |
IEDL.DBID | .~1 |
ISSN | 0010-0277 1873-7838 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 18:31:30 EDT 2025 Sat Sep 27 16:06:21 EDT 2025 Sun Sep 28 00:25:12 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 06:05:04 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:29:11 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:03:44 EDT 2025 Fri Feb 23 02:18:53 EST 2024 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Keywords | Response time and accuracy Number ability Numeracy Diffusion model Individual differences |
Language | English |
License | Copyright © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c561t-cec75ef4c107a7b77cafb5a4ed6b115e8aa26ef825b3e61820737a861e3668573 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/4353499 |
PMID | 25637690 |
PQID | 1660444239 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 22 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4353499 proquest_miscellaneous_1727677689 proquest_miscellaneous_1660444239 pubmed_primary_25637690 crossref_citationtrail_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_12_004 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_12_004 elsevier_sciencedirect_doi_10_1016_j_cognition_2014_12_004 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2015-04-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2015-04-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 04 year: 2015 text: 2015-04-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Netherlands |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Netherlands |
PublicationTitle | Cognition |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Cognition |
PublicationYear | 2015 |
Publisher | Elsevier B.V |
Publisher_xml | – name: Elsevier B.V |
References | Thompson, Siegler (b0215) 2010; 21 Ratcliff, R., & Childers, R. (in press). Individual differences and fitting methods for the two-choice diffusion. Halberda, Ly, Wilmer, Naiman, Germine (b9005) 2012; 109 Ratcliff (b0120) 1978; 85 Ratcliff, Thapar, McKoon (b0155) 2001; 16 Chen, Li (b0020) 2014; 148 Ratcliff, Thapar, McKoon (b0165) 2006; 21 De Smedt, Verschaffel, Ghesquiere (b0030) 2009; 103 Halberda, Mazzocco, Feigenson (b0065) 2008; 455 Wiecki, Sofer, Frank (b0245) 2013; 7 Cohen (b0025) 1992; 112 Gilmore, Attridge, Inglis (b0055) 2011; 64 Brown, Heathcote (b0015) 2008; 57 Gilmore, McCarthy, Spelke (b0060) 2010; 115 Libertus, Feigenson, Halberda (b0085) 2011; 14 Bogacz, Brown, Moehlis, Holmes, Cohen (b0005) 2006; 113 Laming (b0080) 1968 Price, Palmer, Battista, Ansari (b0115) 2012; 140 Voss, Nagler, Lerche (b0230) 2013; 60 Ratcliff, Thapar, McKoon (b0170) 2010; 60 Sasanguie, Defever, Van den Bussche, Reynvoet (b0195) 2011; 136 Ratcliff, Thapar, McKoon (b0160) 2006; 13 . Ratcliff (b0125) 2002; 9 Rinkenauer, Osman, Ulrich, Muller-Gethmann, Mattes (b0190) 2004; 133 Starns, Ratcliff (b0205) 2012; 19 Zebain, Ansari (b0250) 2012; 19 Voss, Voss (b0235) 2007; 39 Ratcliff, Love, Thompson, Opfer (b0145) 2012; 83 White, Ratcliff, Vasey, McKoon (b0240) 2010; 54 Dehaene, Dupoux, Mehler (b0045) 1990; 16 Ratcliff (b0135) 2014; 40 Thapar, Ratcliff, McKoon (b9030) 2003; 18 Ratcliff, McKoon (b0150) 2008; 20 Maloney, Risko, Preston, Ansari, Fugelsang (b0095) 2010; 134 Ratcliff (b0130) 2013; 120 Ratcliff, Thapar, McKoon (b9020) 2004; 50 Dehaene, Dehaene-Lambertz, Cohen (b0040) 1998; 21 Inglis, Attridge, Batchelor, Gilmore (b0075) 2011; 18 Ratcliff, Van Dongen (b9025) 2009; 16 Vandekerckhove, Tuerlinckx (b0225) 2007; 14 Dehaene (b0035) 1997 Brainerd, Gordon (b0010) 1994; 30 Geddes, Ratcliff, Allerhand, Childers, Wright, Frier, Deary (b9000) 2010; 24 Mundy, Gilmore (b0105) 2009; 103 Ratcliff, Rouder (b9010) 1998; 9 Ratcliff, Thapar, McKoon (b9015) 2003; 65 Mazzocco, Feigenson, Halberda (b0100) 2011; 82 Starns, Ratcliff, McKoon (b0210) 2012; 64 Petrov, Van Horn, Ratcliff (b0110) 2011; 18 Usher, McClelland (b0220) 2001; 108 Ratcliff, Thapar, McKoon (b0175) 2011; 140 Ratcliff, Van Zandt, McKoon (b0185) 1999; 106 Holloway, Ansari (b0070) 2009; 103 Ratcliff, Tuerlinckx (b0180) 2002; 9 Starns, Ratcliff (b0200) 2010; 5 Durand, Hulme, Larkin, Snowling (b0050) 2005; 91 Lyons, Beilock (b0090) 2011 Dehaene (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0040) 1998; 21 Petrov (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0110) 2011; 18 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0155) 2001; 16 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0165) 2006; 21 Brainerd (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0010) 1994; 30 Holloway (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0070) 2009; 103 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0145) 2012; 83 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0150) 2008; 20 Gilmore (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0055) 2011; 64 Chen (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0020) 2014; 148 Zebain (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0250) 2012; 19 Mazzocco (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0100) 2011; 82 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0170) 2010; 60 Rinkenauer (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0190) 2004; 133 Voss (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0230) 2013; 60 Dehaene (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0045) 1990; 16 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9015) 2003; 65 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9010) 1998; 9 Vandekerckhove (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0225) 2007; 14 Dehaene (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0035) 1997 Lyons (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0090) 2011 Gilmore (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0060) 2010; 115 Starns (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0205) 2012; 19 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0135) 2014; 40 Thompson (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0215) 2010; 21 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0125) 2002; 9 Mundy (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0105) 2009; 103 Wiecki (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0245) 2013; 7 Geddes (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9000) 2010; 24 Price (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0115) 2012; 140 Durand (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0050) 2005; 91 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9020) 2004; 50 Bogacz (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0005) 2006; 113 Cohen (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0025) 1992; 112 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0140 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0180) 2002; 9 Starns (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0210) 2012; 64 Halberda (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9005) 2012; 109 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0120) 1978; 85 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0185) 1999; 106 Halberda (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0065) 2008; 455 Sasanguie (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0195) 2011; 136 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9025) 2009; 16 Laming (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0080) 1968 Libertus (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0085) 2011; 14 White (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0240) 2010; 54 Maloney (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0095) 2010; 134 Thapar (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9030) 2003; 18 Usher (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0220) 2001; 108 Starns (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0200) 2010; 5 De Smedt (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0030) 2009; 103 Brown (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0015) 2008; 57 Inglis (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0075) 2011; 18 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0130) 2013; 120 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0160) 2006; 13 Ratcliff (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0175) 2011; 140 Voss (10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0235) 2007; 39 22010889 - Dev Sci. 2011 Nov;14(6):1292-300 2144576 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1990 Aug;16(3):626-41 12120790 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2002 Jun;9(2):278-91 26236754 - Decision (Wash D C ). 2015;2015:null 22445770 - Acta Psychol (Amst). 2012 May;140(1):50-7 18776888 - Nature. 2008 Oct 2;455(7213):665-8 15149253 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004 Jun;133(2):261-82 18229471 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2007 Dec;14(6):1011-26 21075357 - Acta Psychol (Amst). 2011 Jan;136(1):73-80 23935581 - Front Neuroinform. 2013 Aug 02;7:14 22188547 - Child Dev. 2012 Jan-Feb;83(1):367-81 24446719 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2014 Apr;40(2):870-88 10378014 - Psychol Rev. 1999 Apr;106(2):261-300 20644108 - Psychol Sci. 2010 Sep;21(9):1274-81 16768580 - Psychol Aging. 2006 Jun;21(2):353-71 23895923 - Exp Psychol. 2013;60(6):385-402 22079870 - Cogn Psychol. 2012 Feb;64(1-2):1-34 22144142 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2012 Feb;19(1):139-45 22016480 - Cereb Cortex. 2012 Sep;22(9):2102-10 18183889 - Behav Res Methods. 2007 Nov;39(4):767-75 18085991 - Neural Comput. 2008 Apr;20(4):873-922 18513738 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2009 May;103(1):17-29 12412886 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2002 Sep;9(3):438-81 21898191 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2011 Dec;18(6):1222-9 23148742 - Psychol Rev. 2013 Jan;120(1):281-92 19285682 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2009 Aug;103(4):469-79 19565683 - Psychol Bull. 1992 Jul;112(1):155-9 11405319 - Psychol Aging. 2001 Jun;16(2):323-41 20545422 - Psychol Aging. 2010 Jun;25(2):377-90 21394547 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2011 Jun;18(3):490-7 20347435 - Cognition. 2010 Jun;115(3):394-406 22033982 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2012 Feb;19(1):93-100 20185118 - Acta Psychol (Amst). 2010 Jun;134(2):154-61 18243170 - Cogn Psychol. 2008 Nov;57(3):153-78 17014301 - Psychol Rev. 2006 Oct;113(4):700-65 11488378 - Psychol Rev. 2001 Jul;108(3):550-92 17201362 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2006 Aug;13(4):626-35 21679173 - Child Dev. 2011 Jul-Aug;82(4):1224-37 21707207 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2011 Aug;140(3):464-87 20431690 - J Math Psychol. 2010 Feb 1;54(1):39-52 21846265 - Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2011 Nov;64(11):2099-109 15890173 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2005 Jun;91(2):113-36 19962693 - Cogn Psychol. 2010 May;60(3):127-57 9720604 - Trends Neurosci. 1998 Aug;21(8):355-61 24583622 - Acta Psychol (Amst). 2014 May;148:163-72 19327782 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2009 Aug;103(4):490-502 |
References_xml | – volume: 83 start-page: 367 year: 2012 end-page: 381 ident: b0145 article-title: Children are not like older adults: A diffusion model analysis of developmental changes in speeded responses publication-title: Child Development – volume: 64 start-page: 2099 year: 2011 end-page: 2109 ident: b0055 article-title: Measuring the approximate number system publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology – volume: 57 start-page: 153 year: 2008 end-page: 178 ident: b0015 article-title: The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 13 start-page: 626 year: 2006 end-page: 635 ident: b0160 article-title: Aging and individual differences in rapid two-choice decisions publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 108 start-page: 550 year: 2001 end-page: 592 ident: b0220 article-title: The time course of perceptual choice: The leaky, competing accumulator model publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 30 start-page: 163 year: 1994 end-page: 177 ident: b0010 article-title: Development of verbatim and gist memory for numbers publication-title: Developmental Psychology – volume: 103 start-page: 17 year: 2009 end-page: 29 ident: b0070 article-title: Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: The numerical distance effect and individual differences in children’s math achievement publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology – volume: 20 start-page: 873 year: 2008 end-page: 922 ident: b0150 article-title: The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks publication-title: Neural Computation – volume: 9 start-page: 438 year: 2002 end-page: 481 ident: b0180 article-title: Estimating the parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – year: 2011 ident: b0090 article-title: Mathematics anxiety: Separating the math from the anxiety publication-title: Cerebral Cortex – reference: Ratcliff, R., & Childers, R. (in press). Individual differences and fitting methods for the two-choice diffusion. – volume: 19 start-page: 93 year: 2012 end-page: 100 ident: b0250 article-title: Differences between literates and illiterates on symbolic but not nonsymbolic numerical magnitude processing publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 54 start-page: 39 year: 2010 end-page: 52 ident: b0240 article-title: Using diffusion models to understand clinical disorders publication-title: Journal of Mathematical Psychology – volume: 7 start-page: 1 year: 2013 end-page: 10 ident: b0245 article-title: HDDM: Hierarchical Bayesian estimation of the drift-diffusion model in python publication-title: Frontiers in Neuroinformatics – volume: 120 start-page: 281 year: 2013 end-page: 292 ident: b0130 article-title: Parameter variability and distributional assumptions in the diffusion model publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 16 start-page: 742 year: 2009 end-page: 751 ident: b9025 article-title: Sleep deprivation affects multiple distinct cognitive processes publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin and Review – volume: 14 start-page: 1292 year: 2011 end-page: 1300 ident: b0085 article-title: Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 115 start-page: 394 year: 2010 end-page: 406 ident: b0060 article-title: Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling publication-title: Cognition – volume: 133 start-page: 261 year: 2004 end-page: 282 ident: b0190 article-title: On the locus of speed–accuracy tradeoff in reaction time: Inferences from the lateralized readiness potential publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General – volume: 103 start-page: 490 year: 2009 end-page: 502 ident: b0105 article-title: Children’s mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representation of number publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology – volume: 16 start-page: 626 year: 1990 end-page: 641 ident: b0045 article-title: Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – year: 1968 ident: b0080 article-title: Information theory of choice reaction time – volume: 91 start-page: 113 year: 2005 end-page: 136 ident: b0050 article-title: The cognitive foundations of reading and arithmetic skills in 7- to 10-year old children publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology – volume: 18 start-page: 490 year: 2011 end-page: 497 ident: b0110 article-title: Dissociable perceptual learning mechanisms revealed by diffusion-model analysis publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 39 start-page: 767 year: 2007 end-page: 775 ident: b0235 article-title: Fast-dm: A free program for efficient diffusion model analysis publication-title: Behavior Research Methods – volume: 19 start-page: 139 year: 2012 end-page: 145 ident: b0205 article-title: Age-related differences in diffusion model boundary optimality with both trial-limited and time-limited tasks publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 134 start-page: 154 year: 2010 end-page: 161 ident: b0095 article-title: Challenging the reliability and validity of cognitive measures: The case of the numerical distance effect publication-title: Acta Psychologica – volume: 18 start-page: 1222 year: 2011 end-page: 1229 ident: b0075 article-title: Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 148 start-page: 163 year: 2014 end-page: 172 ident: b0020 article-title: Association between individual differences in non-symbolic number acuity and math performance: A meta-analysis publication-title: Acta Psychologica – volume: 60 start-page: 385 year: 2013 end-page: 402 ident: b0230 article-title: Diffusion models in experimental psychology: A practical introduction publication-title: Experimental Psychology – volume: 21 start-page: 355 year: 1998 end-page: 361 ident: b0040 article-title: Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain publication-title: Trends in Neuroscience – volume: 24 start-page: 652 year: 2010 end-page: 660 ident: b9000 article-title: Modeling the effects of hypoglycemia on a two-choice task in adult humans publication-title: Neuropsychology – volume: 60 start-page: 127 year: 2010 end-page: 157 ident: b0170 article-title: Individual differences, aging, and IQ in two-choice tasks publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 82 start-page: 1224 year: 2011 end-page: 1237 ident: b0100 article-title: Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalcula) publication-title: Child Development – volume: 16 start-page: 323 year: 2001 end-page: 341 ident: b0155 article-title: The effects of aging on reaction time in a signal detection task publication-title: Psychology and Aging – volume: 85 start-page: 59 year: 1978 end-page: 108 ident: b0120 article-title: A theory of memory retrieval publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 21 start-page: 1274 year: 2010 end-page: 1281 ident: b0215 article-title: Linear numerical magnitude representations aid children’s memory for numbers publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 14 start-page: 1011 year: 2007 end-page: 1026 ident: b0225 article-title: Fitting the Ratcliff diffusion model to experimental data publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 18 start-page: 415 year: 2003 end-page: 429 ident: b9030 article-title: A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging on letter discrimination publication-title: Psychology and Aging – volume: 106 start-page: 261 year: 1999 end-page: 300 ident: b0185 article-title: Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 64 start-page: 1 year: 2012 end-page: 34 ident: b0210 article-title: Evaluating the unequal-variability and dual-process explanations of zROC slopes with response time data and the diffusion model publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 9 start-page: 278 year: 2002 end-page: 291 ident: b0125 article-title: A diffusion model account of reaction time and accuracy in a two choice brightness discrimination task: Fitting real data and failing to fit fake but plausible data publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 103 start-page: 469 year: 2009 end-page: 479 ident: b0030 article-title: The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology – volume: 50 start-page: 408 year: 2004 end-page: 424 ident: b9020 article-title: A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging on recognition memory publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 455 start-page: 665 year: 2008 end-page: 668 ident: b0065 article-title: Individual differences in nonverbal number acuity predict maths achievement publication-title: Nature – volume: 109 start-page: 11116 year: 2012 end-page: 11120 ident: b9005 article-title: Number sense across lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences – volume: 113 start-page: 700 year: 2006 end-page: 765 ident: b0005 article-title: The physics of optimal decision making: A formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced choice tasks publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 140 start-page: 46 year: 2011 end-page: 487 ident: b0175 article-title: Effects of aging and IQ on item and associative memory publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General – volume: 140 start-page: 50 year: 2012 end-page: 57 ident: b0115 article-title: Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: Reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults publication-title: Acta Psychologica – reference: . – volume: 21 start-page: 353 year: 2006 end-page: 371 ident: b0165 article-title: Aging, practice, and perceptual tasks: A diffusion model analysis publication-title: Psychology and Aging – volume: 136 start-page: 73 year: 2011 end-page: 80 ident: b0195 article-title: The reliability of and the relation between non-symbolic numerical distance effects in comparison, same-different judgments and priming publication-title: Acta Psychologica – volume: 40 start-page: 870 year: 2014 end-page: 888 ident: b0135 article-title: Measuring psychometric functions with the diffusion model publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – year: 1997 ident: b0035 article-title: The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics – volume: 112 start-page: 155 year: 1992 end-page: 159 ident: b0025 article-title: A power primer publication-title: Psychological Bulletin – volume: 65 start-page: 523 year: 2003 end-page: 535 ident: b9015 article-title: A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging on brightness discrimination publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics – volume: 9 start-page: 347 year: 1998 end-page: 356 ident: b9010 article-title: Modeling response times for two-choice decisions publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 5 start-page: 377 year: 2010 end-page: 390 ident: b0200 article-title: The effects of aging on the speed–accuracy compromise: Boundary optimality in the diffusion model publication-title: Psychology and Aging – volume: 18 start-page: 490 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0110 article-title: Dissociable perceptual learning mechanisms revealed by diffusion-model analysis publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0079-8 – volume: 106 start-page: 261 year: 1999 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0185 article-title: Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.261 – volume: 64 start-page: 2099 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0055 article-title: Measuring the approximate number system publication-title: Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.574710 – volume: 103 start-page: 17 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0070 article-title: Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: The numerical distance effect and individual differences in children’s math achievement publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.001 – volume: 9 start-page: 347 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9010 article-title: Modeling response times for two-choice decisions publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00067 – volume: 64 start-page: 1 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0210 article-title: Evaluating the unequal-variability and dual-process explanations of zROC slopes with response time data and the diffusion model publication-title: Cognitive Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.10.002 – volume: 24 start-page: 652 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9000 article-title: Modeling the effects of hypoglycemia on a two-choice task in adult humans publication-title: Neuropsychology doi: 10.1037/a0020074 – volume: 82 start-page: 1224 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0100 article-title: Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalcula) publication-title: Child Development doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01608.x – volume: 5 start-page: 377 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0200 article-title: The effects of aging on the speed–accuracy compromise: Boundary optimality in the diffusion model publication-title: Psychology and Aging doi: 10.1037/a0018022 – volume: 455 start-page: 665 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0065 article-title: Individual differences in nonverbal number acuity predict maths achievement publication-title: Nature doi: 10.1038/nature07246 – volume: 83 start-page: 367 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0145 article-title: Children are not like older adults: A diffusion model analysis of developmental changes in speeded responses publication-title: Child Development doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01683.x – volume: 19 start-page: 139 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0205 article-title: Age-related differences in diffusion model boundary optimality with both trial-limited and time-limited tasks publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0189-3 – volume: 148 start-page: 163 year: 2014 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0020 article-title: Association between individual differences in non-symbolic number acuity and math performance: A meta-analysis publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.01.016 – volume: 65 start-page: 523 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9015 article-title: A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging on brightness discrimination publication-title: Perception and Psychophysics doi: 10.3758/BF03194580 – volume: 103 start-page: 469 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0030 article-title: The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.010 – volume: 16 start-page: 323 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0155 article-title: The effects of aging on reaction time in a signal detection task publication-title: Psychology and Aging doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.323 – volume: 40 start-page: 870 year: 2014 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0135 article-title: Measuring psychometric functions with the diffusion model publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 9 start-page: 438 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0180 article-title: Estimating the parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03196302 – volume: 21 start-page: 1274 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0215 article-title: Linear numerical magnitude representations aid children’s memory for numbers publication-title: Psychological Science doi: 10.1177/0956797610378309 – volume: 14 start-page: 1011 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0225 article-title: Fitting the Ratcliff diffusion model to experimental data publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03193087 – volume: 133 start-page: 261 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0190 article-title: On the locus of speed–accuracy tradeoff in reaction time: Inferences from the lateralized readiness potential publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.261 – year: 1997 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0035 – volume: 91 start-page: 113 year: 2005 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0050 article-title: The cognitive foundations of reading and arithmetic skills in 7- to 10-year old children publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.01.003 – volume: 19 start-page: 93 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0250 article-title: Differences between literates and illiterates on symbolic but not nonsymbolic numerical magnitude processing publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0175-9 – volume: 120 start-page: 281 year: 2013 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0130 article-title: Parameter variability and distributional assumptions in the diffusion model publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/a0030775 – volume: 54 start-page: 39 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0240 article-title: Using diffusion models to understand clinical disorders publication-title: Journal of Mathematical Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2010.01.004 – volume: 113 start-page: 700 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0005 article-title: The physics of optimal decision making: A formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced choice tasks publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.4.700 – volume: 13 start-page: 626 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0160 article-title: Aging and individual differences in rapid two-choice decisions publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03193973 – volume: 134 start-page: 154 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0095 article-title: Challenging the reliability and validity of cognitive measures: The case of the numerical distance effect publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.01.006 – year: 1968 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0080 – volume: 21 start-page: 355 year: 1998 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0040 article-title: Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain publication-title: Trends in Neuroscience doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(98)01263-6 – year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0090 article-title: Mathematics anxiety: Separating the math from the anxiety publication-title: Cerebral Cortex – volume: 85 start-page: 59 year: 1978 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0120 article-title: A theory of memory retrieval publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.85.2.59 – volume: 60 start-page: 127 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0170 article-title: Individual differences, aging, and IQ in two-choice tasks publication-title: Cognitive Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.09.001 – volume: 18 start-page: 1222 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0075 article-title: Non-verbal number acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: But only in children publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0154-1 – ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0140 – volume: 50 start-page: 408 year: 2004 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9020 article-title: A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging on recognition memory publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2003.11.002 – volume: 140 start-page: 50 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0115 article-title: Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: Reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.02.008 – volume: 7 start-page: 1 year: 2013 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0245 article-title: HDDM: Hierarchical Bayesian estimation of the drift-diffusion model in python publication-title: Frontiers in Neuroinformatics doi: 10.3389/fninf.2013.00014 – volume: 9 start-page: 278 year: 2002 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0125 article-title: A diffusion model account of reaction time and accuracy in a two choice brightness discrimination task: Fitting real data and failing to fit fake but plausible data publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review doi: 10.3758/BF03196283 – volume: 108 start-page: 550 year: 2001 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0220 article-title: The time course of perceptual choice: The leaky, competing accumulator model publication-title: Psychological Review doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.550 – volume: 16 start-page: 626 year: 1990 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0045 article-title: Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance – volume: 21 start-page: 353 year: 2006 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0165 article-title: Aging, practice, and perceptual tasks: A diffusion model analysis publication-title: Psychology and Aging doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.353 – volume: 39 start-page: 767 year: 2007 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0235 article-title: Fast-dm: A free program for efficient diffusion model analysis publication-title: Behavior Research Methods doi: 10.3758/BF03192967 – volume: 136 start-page: 73 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0195 article-title: The reliability of and the relation between non-symbolic numerical distance effects in comparison, same-different judgments and priming publication-title: Acta Psychologica doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.10.004 – volume: 109 start-page: 11116 year: 2012 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9005 article-title: Number sense across lifespan as revealed by a massive Internet-based sample publication-title: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200196109 – volume: 115 start-page: 394 year: 2010 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0060 article-title: Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and mathematics achievement in the first year of formal schooling publication-title: Cognition doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.02.002 – volume: 16 start-page: 742 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9025 article-title: Sleep deprivation affects multiple distinct cognitive processes publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin and Review doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.4.742 – volume: 14 start-page: 1292 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0085 article-title: Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability publication-title: Developmental Science doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01080.x – volume: 30 start-page: 163 year: 1994 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0010 article-title: Development of verbatim and gist memory for numbers publication-title: Developmental Psychology doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.30.2.163 – volume: 18 start-page: 415 year: 2003 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b9030 article-title: A diffusion model analysis of the effects of aging on letter discrimination publication-title: Psychology and Aging doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.3.415 – volume: 103 start-page: 490 year: 2009 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0105 article-title: Children’s mapping between symbolic and nonsymbolic representation of number publication-title: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.003 – volume: 60 start-page: 385 year: 2013 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0230 article-title: Diffusion models in experimental psychology: A practical introduction publication-title: Experimental Psychology doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000218 – volume: 57 start-page: 153 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0015 article-title: The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation publication-title: Cognitive Psychology doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.12.002 – volume: 112 start-page: 155 year: 1992 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0025 article-title: A power primer publication-title: Psychological Bulletin doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 – volume: 20 start-page: 873 year: 2008 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0150 article-title: The diffusion decision model: Theory and data for two-choice decision tasks publication-title: Neural Computation doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.12-06-420 – volume: 140 start-page: 46 year: 2011 ident: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004_b0175 article-title: Effects of aging and IQ on item and associative memory publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General – reference: 22079870 - Cogn Psychol. 2012 Feb;64(1-2):1-34 – reference: 2144576 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1990 Aug;16(3):626-41 – reference: 24583622 - Acta Psychol (Amst). 2014 May;148:163-72 – reference: 16768580 - Psychol Aging. 2006 Jun;21(2):353-71 – reference: 18085991 - Neural Comput. 2008 Apr;20(4):873-922 – reference: 23148742 - Psychol Rev. 2013 Jan;120(1):281-92 – reference: 18183889 - Behav Res Methods. 2007 Nov;39(4):767-75 – reference: 20644108 - Psychol Sci. 2010 Sep;21(9):1274-81 – reference: 19962693 - Cogn Psychol. 2010 May;60(3):127-57 – reference: 15890173 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2005 Jun;91(2):113-36 – reference: 19327782 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2009 Aug;103(4):490-502 – reference: 19285682 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2009 Aug;103(4):469-79 – reference: 11488378 - Psychol Rev. 2001 Jul;108(3):550-92 – reference: 21679173 - Child Dev. 2011 Jul-Aug;82(4):1224-37 – reference: 20431690 - J Math Psychol. 2010 Feb 1;54(1):39-52 – reference: 9720604 - Trends Neurosci. 1998 Aug;21(8):355-61 – reference: 21898191 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2011 Dec;18(6):1222-9 – reference: 26236754 - Decision (Wash D C ). 2015;2015:null – reference: 18229471 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2007 Dec;14(6):1011-26 – reference: 20185118 - Acta Psychol (Amst). 2010 Jun;134(2):154-61 – reference: 23895923 - Exp Psychol. 2013;60(6):385-402 – reference: 18243170 - Cogn Psychol. 2008 Nov;57(3):153-78 – reference: 22188547 - Child Dev. 2012 Jan-Feb;83(1):367-81 – reference: 11405319 - Psychol Aging. 2001 Jun;16(2):323-41 – reference: 18513738 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2009 May;103(1):17-29 – reference: 20347435 - Cognition. 2010 Jun;115(3):394-406 – reference: 12412886 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2002 Sep;9(3):438-81 – reference: 22016480 - Cereb Cortex. 2012 Sep;22(9):2102-10 – reference: 22144142 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2012 Feb;19(1):139-45 – reference: 22010889 - Dev Sci. 2011 Nov;14(6):1292-300 – reference: 21707207 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2011 Aug;140(3):464-87 – reference: 12120790 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2002 Jun;9(2):278-91 – reference: 22033982 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2012 Feb;19(1):93-100 – reference: 21846265 - Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2011 Nov;64(11):2099-109 – reference: 18776888 - Nature. 2008 Oct 2;455(7213):665-8 – reference: 24446719 - J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2014 Apr;40(2):870-88 – reference: 23935581 - Front Neuroinform. 2013 Aug 02;7:14 – reference: 17014301 - Psychol Rev. 2006 Oct;113(4):700-65 – reference: 15149253 - J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004 Jun;133(2):261-82 – reference: 17201362 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2006 Aug;13(4):626-35 – reference: 19565683 - Psychol Bull. 1992 Jul;112(1):155-9 – reference: 10378014 - Psychol Rev. 1999 Apr;106(2):261-300 – reference: 21394547 - Psychon Bull Rev. 2011 Jun;18(3):490-7 – reference: 20545422 - Psychol Aging. 2010 Jun;25(2):377-90 – reference: 22445770 - Acta Psychol (Amst). 2012 May;140(1):50-7 – reference: 21075357 - Acta Psychol (Amst). 2011 Jan;136(1):73-80 |
SSID | ssj0004278 |
Score | 2.4129627 |
Snippet | •A diffusion model is fit to accuracy and RT distributions for three numeracy tasks.•The model extracts measures of components of the decision-making... In the study of numeracy, some hypotheses have been based on response time (RT) as a dependent variable and some on accuracy, and considerable controversy has... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref elsevier |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 115 |
SubjectTerms | Decision Making Diffusion model Female Humans Individual differences Individuality Male Mathematics Memory - physiology Number ability Numeracy Psychological Theory Reaction Time - physiology Response time and accuracy Young Adult |
Title | Modeling individual differences in response time and accuracy in numeracy |
URI | https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.004 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25637690 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1660444239 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1727677689 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC4353499 |
Volume | 137 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
journalDatabaseRights | – providerCode: PRVESC databaseName: Baden-Württemberg Complete Freedom Collection (Elsevier) customDbUrl: eissn: 1873-7838 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0004278 issn: 0010-0277 databaseCode: GBLVA dateStart: 20110101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://www.sciencedirect.com providerName: Elsevier – providerCode: PRVESC databaseName: Elsevier ScienceDirect Freedom Collection Journals customDbUrl: eissn: 1873-7838 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0004278 issn: 0010-0277 databaseCode: ACRLP dateStart: 19950101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://www.sciencedirect.com providerName: Elsevier – providerCode: PRVESC databaseName: Elsevier SD Freedom Collection Journals [SCFCJ] customDbUrl: eissn: 1873-7838 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0004278 issn: 0010-0277 databaseCode: AIKHN dateStart: 19950101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://www.sciencedirect.com providerName: Elsevier – providerCode: PRVESC databaseName: ScienceDirect Freedom Collection 2013 customDbUrl: eissn: 1873-7838 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0004278 issn: 0010-0277 databaseCode: .~1 dateStart: 19950101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://www.sciencedirect.com providerName: Elsevier – providerCode: PRVLSH databaseName: Elsevier Journals customDbUrl: mediaType: online eissn: 1873-7838 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0004278 issn: 0010-0277 databaseCode: AKRWK dateStart: 19950101 isFulltext: true providerName: Library Specific Holdings |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV07T8MwELYqWFgQb8pLQWINNPErZUMVqAXRiUrdLNu5iiKUVqUdWPjt3OUFBQQDWxLbknO27872d98xdsZbiZOQQjhCRRCKNpehdZEKJWihkrYduZQO9O_7qjsQt0M5bLBOFQtDsMpS9xc6PdfW5ZeLUpoX0_GYYnyJPlQT4xwaSUGcoMT-hXP6_O0D5kGpJApt3MrvK5cwXiVEZ0JEqJHIzwXLjG0_WKjvHuhXIOUny3SzwdZLlzK4Knq9yRqQbbG1WrO9brMeZTyjuPNgXMdfBVVqFFQU-DmYFWBZCCjbfGCzNLDeL2bWv1JpRmGa-LzDBjfXD51uWOZQCD16RvPQg9cSRsLjNs9qp7VH8UsrIFUOnUFIrI0VjHCf6DgoYnPXXNtERcCVSqTmu2wlm2SwzwKfOiFTLoG7WFjQlkdCcg1o71TsRdpkqpKb8SXBOOW5eDYVkuzJ1AI3JHATxQYF3mStuuG04Nj4u8llNTBmaboYtAR_Nz6thtLgYqIbEpvBZPFiIqWIPy_m7V_qoMenNO7SsM5eMfx1r9F_RIXdbjWZXpoYdQUi814uycaPOak3uq0cd58H__mxQ7aGb7IAFh2xlflsAcfoM83dSb4oTtjqVe-u238HL78aFA |
linkProvider | Elsevier |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwpV1LTxsxEB5BOJRLRQuFQB9bieuK7Pq14YZQUVIgJ5C4WbZ3IoLQBqXJgX_PTNa7NIDKobeVH5J3bH8ztme-ATgUvcIrLDEdExCksi9U6nymU4VG6qLvxr7kC_3LkR5cy9836mYNTptYGHarjNhfY_oSrWPJUZTm0cNkwjG-TB9qmHGOlKSU67AhFWFyBzZOhueD0XN4ZN4Acm_5ZLni5hW9dKbMhZrJ5dVgTNr2hpJ6bYS-9KX8SzmdbcHHaFUmJ_XAP8EaVp9hswW3x20YctIzDj1PJm0IVtJkRyGsoOJkVvvLYsIJ5xNXlYkLYTFz4ZFrK47UpO8duD77dXU6SGMahTSQcTRPAwajcCwDnfSc8cYEmgHlJJbakz2IhXO5xjEdFb1AzYTuRhhX6AyF1oUy4gt0qmmFe5CE0ktVCoXC59KhcSKTShgklafzIMsu6EZuNkSOcU51cW8bZ7I72wrcssBtllsSeBd6bceHmmbj_S7HzcTYlRVjSRm83_lnM5WW9hM_krgKp4s_NtOaKfRy0f9HGzL6tKGDGrXZrae_HTWZkITZ_V4XzMrCaBswn_dqTTW5XfJ6k-Uq6AC6_z8_9gM-DK4uL-zFcHR-AJtUo2o_o6_Qmc8W-I1MqLn_HrfIE62jHL8 |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Modeling+individual+differences+in+response+time+and+accuracy+in+numeracy&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.au=Ratcliff%2C+Roger&rft.au=Thompson%2C+Clarissa+A.&rft.au=McKoon%2C+Gail&rft.date=2015-04-01&rft.issn=0010-0277&rft.volume=137&rft.spage=115&rft.epage=136&rft_id=info:doi/10.1016%2Fj.cognition.2014.12.004&rft.externalDBID=n%2Fa&rft.externalDocID=10_1016_j_cognition_2014_12_004 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0010-0277&client=summon |