The Role of Risk Aversion in Non-Conscious Decision Making

To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstraine...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inFrontiers in Psychology Vol. 3; p. 50
Main Authors Wang, Shuo, Krajbich, Ian, Adolphs, Ralph, Tsuchiya, Naotsugu
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published Switzerland Frontiers Media SA 01.01.2012
Frontiers Research Foundation
Frontiers Media S.A
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1664-1078
1664-1078
DOI10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00050

Cover

Abstract To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstrained in many respects, this finding remains debated and other interpretations are possible (e.g., risk aversion, ambiguity aversion, limits of working memory, or insensitivity to reward/punishment can explain the finding of the IGT). Here we devised an improved variant of the IGT in which the deck-payoff contingency switches after subjects repeatedly choose from a good deck, offering the statistical power of repeated within-subject measures based on learning the reward contingencies associated with each deck. We found that participants exhibited low confidence in their choices, as probed with post-decision wagering, despite high accuracy in selecting advantageous decks in the task, which is putative evidence for non-conscious decision making. However, such a behavioral dissociation could also be explained by risk aversion, a tendency to avoid risky decisions under uncertainty. By explicitly measuring risk aversion for each individual, we predicted subjects' post-decision wagering using Bayesian modeling. We found that risk aversion indeed does play a role, but that it did not explain the entire effect. Moreover, independently measured risk aversion was uncorrelated with risk aversion exhibited during our version of the IGT, raising the possibility that the latter risk aversion may be non-conscious. Our findings support the idea that people can make optimal choices without being fully aware of the basis of their decision. We suggest that non-conscious decision making may be mediated by emotional feelings of risk that are based on mechanisms distinct from those that support cognitive assessment of risk.
AbstractList To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstrained in many respects, this finding remains debated and other interpretations are possible (e.g., risk aversion, ambiguity aversion, limits of working memory, or insensitivity to reward/punishment can explain the finding of the IGT). Here we devised an improved variant of the IGT in which the deck-payoff contingency switches after subjects repeatedly choose from a good deck, offering the statistical power of repeated within-subject measures based on learning the reward contingencies associated with each deck. We found that participants exhibited low confidence in their choices, as probed with post-decision wagering, despite high accuracy in selecting advantageous decks in the task, which is putative evidence for non-conscious decision making. However, such a behavioral dissociation could also be explained by risk aversion, a tendency to avoid risky decisions under uncertainty. By explicitly measuring risk aversion for each individual, we predicted subjects' post-decision wagering using Bayesian modeling. We found that risk aversion indeed does play a role, but that it did not explain the entire effect. Moreover, independently measured risk aversion was uncorrelated with risk aversion exhibited during our version of the IGT, raising the possibility that the latter risk aversion may be non-conscious. Our findings support the idea that people can make optimal choices without being fully aware of the basis of their decision. We suggest that non-conscious decision making may be mediated by emotional feelings of risk that are based on mechanisms distinct from those that support cognitive assessment of risk.
To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstrained in many respects, this finding remains debated and other interpretations are possible (e.g., risk aversion, ambiguity aversion, limits of working memory, or insensitivity to reward/punishment can explain the finding of the IGT). Here we devised an improved variant of the IGT in which the deck-payoff contingency switches after subjects repeatedly choose from a good deck, offering the statistical power of repeated within-subject measures based on learning the reward contingencies associated with each deck. We found that participants exhibited low confidence in their choices, as probed with post-decision wagering, despite high accuracy in selecting advantageous decks in the task, which is putative evidence for non-conscious decision making. However, such a behavioral dissociation could also be explained by risk aversion, a tendency to avoid risky decisions under uncertainty. By explicitly measuring risk aversion for each individual, we predicted subjects' post-decision wagering using Bayesian modeling. We found that risk aversion indeed does play a role, but that it did not explain the entire effect. Moreover, independently measured risk aversion was uncorrelated with risk aversion exhibited during our version of the IGT, raising the possibility that the latter risk aversion may be non-conscious. Our findings support the idea that people can make optimal choices without being fully aware of the basis of their decision. We suggest that non-conscious decision making may be mediated by emotional feelings of risk that are based on mechanisms distinct from those that support cognitive assessment of risk.To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstrained in many respects, this finding remains debated and other interpretations are possible (e.g., risk aversion, ambiguity aversion, limits of working memory, or insensitivity to reward/punishment can explain the finding of the IGT). Here we devised an improved variant of the IGT in which the deck-payoff contingency switches after subjects repeatedly choose from a good deck, offering the statistical power of repeated within-subject measures based on learning the reward contingencies associated with each deck. We found that participants exhibited low confidence in their choices, as probed with post-decision wagering, despite high accuracy in selecting advantageous decks in the task, which is putative evidence for non-conscious decision making. However, such a behavioral dissociation could also be explained by risk aversion, a tendency to avoid risky decisions under uncertainty. By explicitly measuring risk aversion for each individual, we predicted subjects' post-decision wagering using Bayesian modeling. We found that risk aversion indeed does play a role, but that it did not explain the entire effect. Moreover, independently measured risk aversion was uncorrelated with risk aversion exhibited during our version of the IGT, raising the possibility that the latter risk aversion may be non-conscious. Our findings support the idea that people can make optimal choices without being fully aware of the basis of their decision. We suggest that non-conscious decision making may be mediated by emotional feelings of risk that are based on mechanisms distinct from those that support cognitive assessment of risk.
To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstrained in many respects, this finding remains debated and other interpretations are possible (e.g., risk aversion, ambiguity aversion, limits of working memory or insensitivity to reward/punishment can explain the finding of the IGT). Here we devised an improved variant of the IGT in which the deck-payoff contingency switches after subjects repeatedly choose from a good deck, offering the statistical power of repeated within-subject measures based on learning the reward contingencies associated with each deck. We found that participants exhibited low confidence in their choices, as probed with post-decision wagering, despite high accuracy in selecting advantageous decks in the task, which is putative evidence for non-conscious decision-making. However, such a behavioral dissociation could also be explained by risk-aversion, a tendency to avoid risky decisions under uncertainty. By explicitly measuring risk aversion for each individual, we predicted subjects’ post-decision wagering using Bayesian modeling. We found that risk aversion indeed does play a role, but that it does not explain the entire effect. Moreover, independently-measured risk aversion was uncorrelated with risk aversion exhibited during our version of the IGT, raising the possibility that the latter risk aversion may be non-conscious. Our findings support the idea that people can make optimal choices without being fully aware of the basis of their decision. We suggest that non-conscious decision-making may be mediated by emotional feelings of risk that are based on mechanisms distinct from those that support cognitive assessment of risk.
Author Ian eKrajbich
Ralph eAdolphs
Shuo eWang
Naotsugu eTsuchiya
AuthorAffiliation 6 Japan Science and Technology Tokyo-to, Japan
7 School of Psychology and Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing, and Health Sciences, Monash University Clayton, VIC, Australia
4 Brain Science Institute, RIKEN Saitama-ken, Japan
2 Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology Pasadena, CA, USA
5 Advanced Telecommunication Research Kyoto-fu, Japan
1 Computation and Neural Systems, California Institute of Technology Pasadena, CA, USA
3 Department of Economics, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland
AuthorAffiliation_xml – name: 1 Computation and Neural Systems, California Institute of Technology Pasadena, CA, USA
– name: 6 Japan Science and Technology Tokyo-to, Japan
– name: 4 Brain Science Institute, RIKEN Saitama-ken, Japan
– name: 7 School of Psychology and Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing, and Health Sciences, Monash University Clayton, VIC, Australia
– name: 2 Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology Pasadena, CA, USA
– name: 3 Department of Economics, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland
– name: 5 Advanced Telecommunication Research Kyoto-fu, Japan
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Shuo
  surname: Wang
  fullname: Wang, Shuo
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Ian
  surname: Krajbich
  fullname: Krajbich, Ian
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Ralph
  surname: Adolphs
  fullname: Adolphs, Ralph
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Naotsugu
  surname: Tsuchiya
  fullname: Tsuchiya, Naotsugu
BackLink https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1870021790882151296$$DView record in CiNii
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22375133$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9Uk1PHCEYJo1Nteq9p2YOTXqa7QsMA_TQxKz9MLE1MfZMGOadFZ2FLcya-O9ld7XRHnoBwvt8hOfhLdkLMSAh7yjMOFf607DK94sZA8pmACDgFTmgbdvUFKTae3beJ8c53xQINMAA2BuyzxiXgnJ-QD5fXWN1GUes4lBd-nxbndxhyj6GyofqVwz1PIbsfFzn6hSd305-2lsfFkfk9WDHjMeP-yH5_e3r1fxHfX7x_Wx-cl47IdqpZsiKVWspcq3Buh5V03KqtQQHTHOu-65z2qqBUS255g0iUNENVA2F6vghOdvp9tHemFXyS5vuTbTebC9iWhibJu9GNN0gAFFJbalqdN8oroV2rVDl3Y2TTdH6stNarbsl9g7DlOz4QvTlJPhrs4h3hjPVKsGLwMdHgRT_rDFPZumzw3G0AUtGRjMhqeRMFOT751Z_PZ6yL4B2B3Ap5pxwMM5PdioBF2c_Ggpm07PZ9mw2PZttz4UI_xCftP9D-bCjBO-LzWalSpbPQKUGpRgVlOmWPwDYi7M5
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_36834_cmej_73643
crossref_primary_10_1590_0102_3772e33415
crossref_primary_10_1111_medu_15466
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jclinane_2014_01_003
crossref_primary_10_1038_s44220_024_00229_x
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_thorsurg_2012_07_005
crossref_primary_10_1038_s44184_024_00074_x
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2023_1249008
crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_phyto_082712_102356
crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1217513110
crossref_primary_10_2196_47979
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11409_024_09411_z
crossref_primary_10_2478_ppb_2013_0011
crossref_primary_10_3390_w13162226
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright Copyright © 2012 Wang, Krajbich, Adolphs and Tsuchiya. 2012
Copyright_xml – notice: Copyright © 2012 Wang, Krajbich, Adolphs and Tsuchiya. 2012
DBID RYH
AAYXX
CITATION
NPM
7X8
5PM
DOA
DOI 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00050
DatabaseName CiNii Complete
CrossRef
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList PubMed
MEDLINE - Academic


Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: DOA
  name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  url: https://www.doaj.org/
  sourceTypes: Open Website
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1664-1078
ExternalDocumentID oai_doaj_org_article_bf50ee879a1849d483959c6582004c74
PMC3286853
22375133
10_3389_fpsyg_2012_00050
Genre Journal Article
GroupedDBID 53G
5VS
9T4
AAFWJ
AAKDD
ABIVO
ACGFO
ACGFS
ACHQT
ACXDI
ADBBV
ADRAZ
AEGXH
AFPKN
AIAGR
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
AOIJS
BAWUL
BCNDV
DIK
EBS
EJD
F5P
GROUPED_DOAJ
GX1
HYE
KQ8
M48
M~E
O5R
O5S
OK1
P2P
PGMZT
RNS
RPM
RYH
AAYXX
CITATION
EMOBN
IPNFZ
RIG
IAO
ICO
IEA
IHR
IHW
IPY
NPM
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c556t-2e25136a1e3990acde846319970c029339dbbc9a8f21973934ee015bf18fe25c3
IEDL.DBID M48
ISSN 1664-1078
IngestDate Wed Aug 27 01:30:17 EDT 2025
Thu Aug 21 18:13:59 EDT 2025
Fri Sep 05 06:37:08 EDT 2025
Thu Jan 02 22:09:31 EST 2025
Thu Apr 24 22:55:49 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 01:44:46 EDT 2025
Thu Jun 26 22:38:58 EDT 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Keywords risk aversion
consciousness
confidence
decision making
post-decision wagering
Language English
License This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial License, which permits non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c556t-2e25136a1e3990acde846319970c029339dbbc9a8f21973934ee015bf18fe25c3
Notes ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 23
Reviewed by: Christoph T. Weidemann, Swansea University, UK; Stephen M. Fleming, New York University, USA; Eyal Ert, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel; Navindra Persaud, St Michael’s Hospital, Canada
Edited by: Eldad Yechiam, Technion – Israel Institute of Technology, Israel
This article was submitted to Frontiers in Cognitive Science, a specialty of Frontiers in Psychology.
OpenAccessLink http://journals.scholarsportal.info/openUrl.xqy?doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00050
PMID 22375133
PQID 925717325
PQPubID 23479
ParticipantIDs doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_bf50ee879a1849d483959c6582004c74
pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_3286853
proquest_miscellaneous_925717325
pubmed_primary_22375133
crossref_citationtrail_10_3389_fpsyg_2012_00050
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2012_00050
nii_cinii_1870021790882151296
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate 2012-01-01
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2012-01-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 01
  year: 2012
  text: 2012-01-01
  day: 01
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace Switzerland
PublicationPlace_xml – name: Switzerland
PublicationTitle Frontiers in Psychology
PublicationTitleAlternate Front Psychol
PublicationYear 2012
Publisher Frontiers Media SA
Frontiers Research Foundation
Frontiers Media S.A
Publisher_xml – name: Frontiers Media SA
– name: Frontiers Research Foundation
– name: Frontiers Media S.A
References 16504951 - Neuron. 2006 Mar 2;49(5):765-75
18482859 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Jun;12(6):209-10; author reply 210
19836292 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2009 Dec;13(12):517-23
20133167 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Dec;19(4):1069-78
15738960 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005 Mar;6(3):247-55
12214432 - Psychol Assess. 2002 Sep;14(3):253-62
11239442 - Neuron. 2001 Feb;29(2):537-45
19812332 - J Neurosci. 2009 Oct 7;29(40):12574-83
15557557 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004 Nov 30;101(48):16709-10
16286932 - Nat Neurosci. 2005 Dec;8(12):1704-11
12003856 - Cereb Cortex. 2002 Jun;12(6):565-74
15928095 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jun 7;102(23):8351-6
17237774 - Nat Neurosci. 2007 Feb;10(2):257-61
15808493 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Apr;9(4):159-62; discussion 162-4
19828331 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Jun;19(2):674-81
8039375 - Cognition. 1994 Apr-Jun;50(1-3):7-15
18178511 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Feb;12(2):54-8
16116449 - Nat Neurosci. 2005 Sep;8(9):1220-7
15501919 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004 Nov 9;101(45):16075-80
12649484 - Science. 2003 Mar 21;299(5614):1898-902
17259960 - Nat Neurosci. 2007 Feb;10(2):140-1
18835129 - Neural Netw. 2008 Nov;21(9):1247-60
11583523 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2001 Nov;80(3):201-24
10377356 - J Neurosci. 1999 Jul 1;19(13):5473-81
10526345 - Nat Neurosci. 1999 Nov;2(11):1032-7
10731224 - Cereb Cortex. 2000 Mar;10(3):295-307
20005133 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Mar;19(1):352-63
10519925 - Cognition. 1999 Oct 26;72(3):269-304
16197997 - Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(2):239-71
7566086 - Nature. 1995 Sep 28;377(6547):336-8
16484496 - Science. 2006 Feb 17;311(5763):1005-7
11448261 - JAMA. 2001 Jul 11;286(2):141-4
15653813 - Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2004 Sep;3(3):159-72
19228971 - J Neurosci. 2009 Feb 18;29(7):2188-92
11316014 - Psychol Bull. 2001 Mar;127(2):267-86
17122317 - J Neurophysiol. 2007 Feb;97(2):1621-32
16899731 - J Neurosci. 2006 Aug 9;26(32):8360-7
9036851 - Science. 1997 Feb 28;275(5304):1293-5
19423820 - Science. 2009 May 8;324(5928):759-64
11691967 - Science. 2001 Nov 2;294(5544):980-2
17362380 - Psychol Sci. 2007 Jan;18(1):64-71
16339445 - Science. 2005 Dec 9;310(5754):1680-3
17403642 - Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007 May 29;362(1481):857-75
20888231 - Curr Biol. 2010 Oct 26;20(20):1823-9
11697867 - Conscious Cogn. 2001 Sep;10(3):294-340
15588617 - Neuroimage. 2005 Jan 1;24(1):253-9
12403997 - Nat Neurosci. 2002 Nov;5(11):1103-4; author reply 1104
16880132 - Neuron. 2006 Aug 3;51(3):381-90
References_xml – reference: 11583523 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2001 Nov;80(3):201-24
– reference: 9036851 - Science. 1997 Feb 28;275(5304):1293-5
– reference: 19828331 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Jun;19(2):674-81
– reference: 11691967 - Science. 2001 Nov 2;294(5544):980-2
– reference: 15653813 - Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2004 Sep;3(3):159-72
– reference: 10731224 - Cereb Cortex. 2000 Mar;10(3):295-307
– reference: 11316014 - Psychol Bull. 2001 Mar;127(2):267-86
– reference: 19836292 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2009 Dec;13(12):517-23
– reference: 16484496 - Science. 2006 Feb 17;311(5763):1005-7
– reference: 16504951 - Neuron. 2006 Mar 2;49(5):765-75
– reference: 16286932 - Nat Neurosci. 2005 Dec;8(12):1704-11
– reference: 10526345 - Nat Neurosci. 1999 Nov;2(11):1032-7
– reference: 11697867 - Conscious Cogn. 2001 Sep;10(3):294-340
– reference: 18482859 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Jun;12(6):209-10; author reply 210
– reference: 15928095 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jun 7;102(23):8351-6
– reference: 16880132 - Neuron. 2006 Aug 3;51(3):381-90
– reference: 12403997 - Nat Neurosci. 2002 Nov;5(11):1103-4; author reply 1104
– reference: 12214432 - Psychol Assess. 2002 Sep;14(3):253-62
– reference: 11239442 - Neuron. 2001 Feb;29(2):537-45
– reference: 19423820 - Science. 2009 May 8;324(5928):759-64
– reference: 16116449 - Nat Neurosci. 2005 Sep;8(9):1220-7
– reference: 15501919 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004 Nov 9;101(45):16075-80
– reference: 10519925 - Cognition. 1999 Oct 26;72(3):269-304
– reference: 17259960 - Nat Neurosci. 2007 Feb;10(2):140-1
– reference: 15588617 - Neuroimage. 2005 Jan 1;24(1):253-9
– reference: 18178511 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Feb;12(2):54-8
– reference: 17403642 - Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007 May 29;362(1481):857-75
– reference: 10377356 - J Neurosci. 1999 Jul 1;19(13):5473-81
– reference: 11448261 - JAMA. 2001 Jul 11;286(2):141-4
– reference: 15738960 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005 Mar;6(3):247-55
– reference: 7566086 - Nature. 1995 Sep 28;377(6547):336-8
– reference: 20133167 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Dec;19(4):1069-78
– reference: 19228971 - J Neurosci. 2009 Feb 18;29(7):2188-92
– reference: 17122317 - J Neurophysiol. 2007 Feb;97(2):1621-32
– reference: 17237774 - Nat Neurosci. 2007 Feb;10(2):257-61
– reference: 18835129 - Neural Netw. 2008 Nov;21(9):1247-60
– reference: 16899731 - J Neurosci. 2006 Aug 9;26(32):8360-7
– reference: 8039375 - Cognition. 1994 Apr-Jun;50(1-3):7-15
– reference: 15808493 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Apr;9(4):159-62; discussion 162-4
– reference: 19812332 - J Neurosci. 2009 Oct 7;29(40):12574-83
– reference: 12003856 - Cereb Cortex. 2002 Jun;12(6):565-74
– reference: 17362380 - Psychol Sci. 2007 Jan;18(1):64-71
– reference: 20005133 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Mar;19(1):352-63
– reference: 16197997 - Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(2):239-71
– reference: 15557557 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004 Nov 30;101(48):16709-10
– reference: 16339445 - Science. 2005 Dec 9;310(5754):1680-3
– reference: 20888231 - Curr Biol. 2010 Oct 26;20(20):1823-9
– reference: 12649484 - Science. 2003 Mar 21;299(5614):1898-902
SSID ssj0000402002
Score 2.0530546
Snippet To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa...
SourceID doaj
pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
nii
SourceType Open Website
Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 50
SubjectTerms BF1-990
confidence
Consciousness
decision making
Post-decision wagering
Psychology
risk aversion
SummonAdditionalLinks – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  dbid: DOA
  link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV09T8NADLUQUxfENwGKbmBhiJo0ySVhA0RVMTBRqdspcXxQCUIFBan_HvuSVi1CsLB0aBP18ny130tebYBzyiwiodiqdODHRNrPkApfIzG51RQnlXNb3OvhKL4bJ-OVUV_iCWvaAzfA9UqbBERZmhesRfIq5oKe5Mh1U8KLqesEymVsRUy5HCyySKw78lySVVjes9P3-aNYuVyDTvmb_Uodcu36ubrUk8lPTPO7YXKlAg22YauljuqqWfIObFC9C51lBpvvwSUHXYlfUL1aJaZxVXw298PUpFYs9H1Wv1zzWO2rqh2u47-4eVT7MBrcPtwM_XY4go9Jomd-n5iZRLoIiSlGUGBFzCQisY0EGHANj_KqLDEvMss5SdrecRy49Jc2zCyfitEBbPL30hGoFGUIetQPUQdxVAY5os7Ikk3QUlaGHvQWUBlsO4fLAItnwwpCwDUOXCPgGgeuBxfLM6ZN14xfjr0W9JfHSb9r9wbvAtPuAvPXLvCgy7Hj1clryDlIpJZYuBpGoz1Qi6ga_v3IQ5GiJkbb5JyzwjTqJx4cNkFeroSZUyrjbzxI18K_ttT1T-rJk2vRHfUzzUTo-D-u7QQ6glZz3-cUNmdvH9RlJjQrz9ym_wK05QJv
  priority: 102
  providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals
Title The Role of Risk Aversion in Non-Conscious Decision Making
URI https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1870021790882151296
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22375133
https://www.proquest.com/docview/925717325
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC3286853
https://doaj.org/article/bf50ee879a1849d483959c6582004c74
Volume 3
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
journalDatabaseRights – providerCode: PRVAFT
  databaseName: Open Access Digital Library
  customDbUrl:
  eissn: 1664-1078
  dateEnd: 99991231
  omitProxy: true
  ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002
  issn: 1664-1078
  databaseCode: KQ8
  dateStart: 20100101
  isFulltext: true
  titleUrlDefault: http://grweb.coalliance.org/oadl/oadl.html
  providerName: Colorado Alliance of Research Libraries
– providerCode: PRVAON
  databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals
  customDbUrl:
  eissn: 1664-1078
  dateEnd: 99991231
  omitProxy: true
  ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002
  issn: 1664-1078
  databaseCode: DOA
  dateStart: 20100101
  isFulltext: true
  titleUrlDefault: https://www.doaj.org/
  providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals
– providerCode: PRVBFR
  databaseName: Free Medical Journals
  customDbUrl:
  eissn: 1664-1078
  dateEnd: 99991231
  omitProxy: true
  ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002
  issn: 1664-1078
  databaseCode: DIK
  dateStart: 20100101
  isFulltext: true
  titleUrlDefault: http://www.freemedicaljournals.com
  providerName: Flying Publisher
– providerCode: PRVFQY
  databaseName: GFMER Free Medical Journals
  customDbUrl:
  eissn: 1664-1078
  dateEnd: 99991231
  omitProxy: true
  ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002
  issn: 1664-1078
  databaseCode: GX1
  dateStart: 0
  isFulltext: true
  titleUrlDefault: http://www.gfmer.ch/Medical_journals/Free_medical.php
  providerName: Geneva Foundation for Medical Education and Research
– providerCode: PRVHPJ
  databaseName: ROAD: Directory of Open Access Scholarly Resources (ISSN International Center)
  customDbUrl:
  eissn: 1664-1078
  dateEnd: 99991231
  omitProxy: true
  ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002
  issn: 1664-1078
  databaseCode: M~E
  dateStart: 20100101
  isFulltext: true
  titleUrlDefault: https://road.issn.org
  providerName: ISSN International Centre
– providerCode: PRVAQN
  databaseName: PubMed Central
  customDbUrl:
  eissn: 1664-1078
  dateEnd: 99991231
  omitProxy: true
  ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002
  issn: 1664-1078
  databaseCode: RPM
  dateStart: 20100101
  isFulltext: true
  titleUrlDefault: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/
  providerName: National Library of Medicine
– providerCode: PRVFZP
  databaseName: Scholars Portal Journals: Open Access
  customDbUrl:
  eissn: 1664-1078
  dateEnd: 20250131
  omitProxy: true
  ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002
  issn: 1664-1078
  databaseCode: M48
  dateStart: 20101201
  isFulltext: true
  titleUrlDefault: http://journals.scholarsportal.info
  providerName: Scholars Portal
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwhV1Lb9RADLZQe-kFUV4bSqs5cOEQNs9JUgmhtlCtkNpDxUp7GyVeT0lZJe3utmL_PfYkXbqowCWHJCNZn8f254ljA7yj3CISSlmVDvyESPs5UulrJCa3mpJ06qotzvVonHydpJPfv0f3AC4eTe1kntR4Pvvw82b1iQ3-o2ScHG-H9nqxupQqLdd7UxL4bY5Lkezxs57sO78sqZKU8wz-tnAjNrkW_hxxmrp-jH3-WUT5ICqdPoOnPZ1UR53-d-HJVfkcdtZebfUCDnkjqIt2Rqq16qJe_FBHd90Zmaobdd42vszsRCmFVZ_7gTvqzM2oegnj0y_fTkZ-PzDBxzTVSz8iZiuxLkNi2hGUOCVmF7GUkgQYcFyPi2lVYVHmlv2UtMJj3TAdqGyYW16K8SvYatqGBqAylMHocRSiDpK4CgpEnZMlm6KlvAo9GN5DZbDvJi5DLWaGswoB1zhwjYBrHLgevF-vuO46afzj3WNBf_2e9MB2N9r5pelNylQ2DYjyrCg5Sy2mCVO9tEBmVGL4mCUe7LPuWDq5huyXJP2Ssq6O5WgP1L1WDduUfCgpG2K0TcF-LMziKPXgdafktSTMpjIZieNBtqH-DVE3nzT1d9e2O45yzeTozX_E2oMdAaI75nkLW8v5Le0z8VlWB-7A4MDt6l-RWPzx
linkProvider Scholars Portal
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+Role+of+Risk+Aversion+in+Non-Conscious+Decision+Making&rft.jtitle=Frontiers+in+Psychology&rft.au=Shuo+eWang&rft.au=Ian+eKrajbich&rft.au=Ralph+eAdolphs&rft.au=Naotsugu+eTsuchiya&rft.date=2012-01-01&rft.pub=Frontiers+Media+SA&rft.eissn=1664-1078&rft.volume=3&rft_id=info:doi/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2012.00050
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon