The Role of Risk Aversion in Non-Conscious Decision Making
To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstraine...
Saved in:
Published in | Frontiers in Psychology Vol. 3; p. 50 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Switzerland
Frontiers Media SA
01.01.2012
Frontiers Research Foundation Frontiers Media S.A |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1664-1078 1664-1078 |
DOI | 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00050 |
Cover
Abstract | To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstrained in many respects, this finding remains debated and other interpretations are possible (e.g., risk aversion, ambiguity aversion, limits of working memory, or insensitivity to reward/punishment can explain the finding of the IGT). Here we devised an improved variant of the IGT in which the deck-payoff contingency switches after subjects repeatedly choose from a good deck, offering the statistical power of repeated within-subject measures based on learning the reward contingencies associated with each deck. We found that participants exhibited low confidence in their choices, as probed with post-decision wagering, despite high accuracy in selecting advantageous decks in the task, which is putative evidence for non-conscious decision making. However, such a behavioral dissociation could also be explained by risk aversion, a tendency to avoid risky decisions under uncertainty. By explicitly measuring risk aversion for each individual, we predicted subjects' post-decision wagering using Bayesian modeling. We found that risk aversion indeed does play a role, but that it did not explain the entire effect. Moreover, independently measured risk aversion was uncorrelated with risk aversion exhibited during our version of the IGT, raising the possibility that the latter risk aversion may be non-conscious. Our findings support the idea that people can make optimal choices without being fully aware of the basis of their decision. We suggest that non-conscious decision making may be mediated by emotional feelings of risk that are based on mechanisms distinct from those that support cognitive assessment of risk. |
---|---|
AbstractList | To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstrained in many respects, this finding remains debated and other interpretations are possible (e.g., risk aversion, ambiguity aversion, limits of working memory, or insensitivity to reward/punishment can explain the finding of the IGT). Here we devised an improved variant of the IGT in which the deck-payoff contingency switches after subjects repeatedly choose from a good deck, offering the statistical power of repeated within-subject measures based on learning the reward contingencies associated with each deck. We found that participants exhibited low confidence in their choices, as probed with post-decision wagering, despite high accuracy in selecting advantageous decks in the task, which is putative evidence for non-conscious decision making. However, such a behavioral dissociation could also be explained by risk aversion, a tendency to avoid risky decisions under uncertainty. By explicitly measuring risk aversion for each individual, we predicted subjects' post-decision wagering using Bayesian modeling. We found that risk aversion indeed does play a role, but that it did not explain the entire effect. Moreover, independently measured risk aversion was uncorrelated with risk aversion exhibited during our version of the IGT, raising the possibility that the latter risk aversion may be non-conscious. Our findings support the idea that people can make optimal choices without being fully aware of the basis of their decision. We suggest that non-conscious decision making may be mediated by emotional feelings of risk that are based on mechanisms distinct from those that support cognitive assessment of risk. To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstrained in many respects, this finding remains debated and other interpretations are possible (e.g., risk aversion, ambiguity aversion, limits of working memory, or insensitivity to reward/punishment can explain the finding of the IGT). Here we devised an improved variant of the IGT in which the deck-payoff contingency switches after subjects repeatedly choose from a good deck, offering the statistical power of repeated within-subject measures based on learning the reward contingencies associated with each deck. We found that participants exhibited low confidence in their choices, as probed with post-decision wagering, despite high accuracy in selecting advantageous decks in the task, which is putative evidence for non-conscious decision making. However, such a behavioral dissociation could also be explained by risk aversion, a tendency to avoid risky decisions under uncertainty. By explicitly measuring risk aversion for each individual, we predicted subjects' post-decision wagering using Bayesian modeling. We found that risk aversion indeed does play a role, but that it did not explain the entire effect. Moreover, independently measured risk aversion was uncorrelated with risk aversion exhibited during our version of the IGT, raising the possibility that the latter risk aversion may be non-conscious. Our findings support the idea that people can make optimal choices without being fully aware of the basis of their decision. We suggest that non-conscious decision making may be mediated by emotional feelings of risk that are based on mechanisms distinct from those that support cognitive assessment of risk.To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstrained in many respects, this finding remains debated and other interpretations are possible (e.g., risk aversion, ambiguity aversion, limits of working memory, or insensitivity to reward/punishment can explain the finding of the IGT). Here we devised an improved variant of the IGT in which the deck-payoff contingency switches after subjects repeatedly choose from a good deck, offering the statistical power of repeated within-subject measures based on learning the reward contingencies associated with each deck. We found that participants exhibited low confidence in their choices, as probed with post-decision wagering, despite high accuracy in selecting advantageous decks in the task, which is putative evidence for non-conscious decision making. However, such a behavioral dissociation could also be explained by risk aversion, a tendency to avoid risky decisions under uncertainty. By explicitly measuring risk aversion for each individual, we predicted subjects' post-decision wagering using Bayesian modeling. We found that risk aversion indeed does play a role, but that it did not explain the entire effect. Moreover, independently measured risk aversion was uncorrelated with risk aversion exhibited during our version of the IGT, raising the possibility that the latter risk aversion may be non-conscious. Our findings support the idea that people can make optimal choices without being fully aware of the basis of their decision. We suggest that non-conscious decision making may be mediated by emotional feelings of risk that are based on mechanisms distinct from those that support cognitive assessment of risk. To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), in which people often learn to choose advantageously without appearing to know why. However, because the IGT is unconstrained in many respects, this finding remains debated and other interpretations are possible (e.g., risk aversion, ambiguity aversion, limits of working memory or insensitivity to reward/punishment can explain the finding of the IGT). Here we devised an improved variant of the IGT in which the deck-payoff contingency switches after subjects repeatedly choose from a good deck, offering the statistical power of repeated within-subject measures based on learning the reward contingencies associated with each deck. We found that participants exhibited low confidence in their choices, as probed with post-decision wagering, despite high accuracy in selecting advantageous decks in the task, which is putative evidence for non-conscious decision-making. However, such a behavioral dissociation could also be explained by risk-aversion, a tendency to avoid risky decisions under uncertainty. By explicitly measuring risk aversion for each individual, we predicted subjects’ post-decision wagering using Bayesian modeling. We found that risk aversion indeed does play a role, but that it does not explain the entire effect. Moreover, independently-measured risk aversion was uncorrelated with risk aversion exhibited during our version of the IGT, raising the possibility that the latter risk aversion may be non-conscious. Our findings support the idea that people can make optimal choices without being fully aware of the basis of their decision. We suggest that non-conscious decision-making may be mediated by emotional feelings of risk that are based on mechanisms distinct from those that support cognitive assessment of risk. |
Author | Ian eKrajbich Ralph eAdolphs Shuo eWang Naotsugu eTsuchiya |
AuthorAffiliation | 6 Japan Science and Technology Tokyo-to, Japan 7 School of Psychology and Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing, and Health Sciences, Monash University Clayton, VIC, Australia 4 Brain Science Institute, RIKEN Saitama-ken, Japan 2 Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology Pasadena, CA, USA 5 Advanced Telecommunication Research Kyoto-fu, Japan 1 Computation and Neural Systems, California Institute of Technology Pasadena, CA, USA 3 Department of Economics, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 1 Computation and Neural Systems, California Institute of Technology Pasadena, CA, USA – name: 6 Japan Science and Technology Tokyo-to, Japan – name: 4 Brain Science Institute, RIKEN Saitama-ken, Japan – name: 7 School of Psychology and Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing, and Health Sciences, Monash University Clayton, VIC, Australia – name: 2 Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology Pasadena, CA, USA – name: 3 Department of Economics, University of Zurich Zurich, Switzerland – name: 5 Advanced Telecommunication Research Kyoto-fu, Japan |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Shuo surname: Wang fullname: Wang, Shuo – sequence: 2 givenname: Ian surname: Krajbich fullname: Krajbich, Ian – sequence: 3 givenname: Ralph surname: Adolphs fullname: Adolphs, Ralph – sequence: 4 givenname: Naotsugu surname: Tsuchiya fullname: Tsuchiya, Naotsugu |
BackLink | https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1870021790882151296$$DView record in CiNii https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22375133$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9Uk1PHCEYJo1Nteq9p2YOTXqa7QsMA_TQxKz9MLE1MfZMGOadFZ2FLcya-O9ld7XRHnoBwvt8hOfhLdkLMSAh7yjMOFf607DK94sZA8pmACDgFTmgbdvUFKTae3beJ8c53xQINMAA2BuyzxiXgnJ-QD5fXWN1GUes4lBd-nxbndxhyj6GyofqVwz1PIbsfFzn6hSd305-2lsfFkfk9WDHjMeP-yH5_e3r1fxHfX7x_Wx-cl47IdqpZsiKVWspcq3Buh5V03KqtQQHTHOu-65z2qqBUS255g0iUNENVA2F6vghOdvp9tHemFXyS5vuTbTebC9iWhibJu9GNN0gAFFJbalqdN8oroV2rVDl3Y2TTdH6stNarbsl9g7DlOz4QvTlJPhrs4h3hjPVKsGLwMdHgRT_rDFPZumzw3G0AUtGRjMhqeRMFOT751Z_PZ6yL4B2B3Ap5pxwMM5PdioBF2c_Ggpm07PZ9mw2PZttz4UI_xCftP9D-bCjBO-LzWalSpbPQKUGpRgVlOmWPwDYi7M5 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_36834_cmej_73643 crossref_primary_10_1590_0102_3772e33415 crossref_primary_10_1111_medu_15466 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jclinane_2014_01_003 crossref_primary_10_1038_s44220_024_00229_x crossref_primary_10_1016_j_thorsurg_2012_07_005 crossref_primary_10_1038_s44184_024_00074_x crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2023_1249008 crossref_primary_10_1146_annurev_phyto_082712_102356 crossref_primary_10_1073_pnas_1217513110 crossref_primary_10_2196_47979 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11409_024_09411_z crossref_primary_10_2478_ppb_2013_0011 crossref_primary_10_3390_w13162226 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Copyright © 2012 Wang, Krajbich, Adolphs and Tsuchiya. 2012 |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Copyright © 2012 Wang, Krajbich, Adolphs and Tsuchiya. 2012 |
DBID | RYH AAYXX CITATION NPM 7X8 5PM DOA |
DOI | 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00050 |
DatabaseName | CiNii Complete CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1664-1078 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_bf50ee879a1849d483959c6582004c74 PMC3286853 22375133 10_3389_fpsyg_2012_00050 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | 53G 5VS 9T4 AAFWJ AAKDD ABIVO ACGFO ACGFS ACHQT ACXDI ADBBV ADRAZ AEGXH AFPKN AIAGR ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS AOIJS BAWUL BCNDV DIK EBS EJD F5P GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 HYE KQ8 M48 M~E O5R O5S OK1 P2P PGMZT RNS RPM RYH AAYXX CITATION EMOBN IPNFZ RIG IAO ICO IEA IHR IHW IPY NPM 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c556t-2e25136a1e3990acde846319970c029339dbbc9a8f21973934ee015bf18fe25c3 |
IEDL.DBID | M48 |
ISSN | 1664-1078 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:30:17 EDT 2025 Thu Aug 21 18:13:59 EDT 2025 Fri Sep 05 06:37:08 EDT 2025 Thu Jan 02 22:09:31 EST 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:55:49 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 01:44:46 EDT 2025 Thu Jun 26 22:38:58 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Keywords | risk aversion consciousness confidence decision making post-decision wagering |
Language | English |
License | This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non Commercial License, which permits non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c556t-2e25136a1e3990acde846319970c029339dbbc9a8f21973934ee015bf18fe25c3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 Reviewed by: Christoph T. Weidemann, Swansea University, UK; Stephen M. Fleming, New York University, USA; Eyal Ert, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel; Navindra Persaud, St Michael’s Hospital, Canada Edited by: Eldad Yechiam, Technion – Israel Institute of Technology, Israel This article was submitted to Frontiers in Cognitive Science, a specialty of Frontiers in Psychology. |
OpenAccessLink | http://journals.scholarsportal.info/openUrl.xqy?doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00050 |
PMID | 22375133 |
PQID | 925717325 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_bf50ee879a1849d483959c6582004c74 pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_3286853 proquest_miscellaneous_925717325 pubmed_primary_22375133 crossref_citationtrail_10_3389_fpsyg_2012_00050 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2012_00050 nii_cinii_1870021790882151296 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2012-01-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2012-01-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 01 year: 2012 text: 2012-01-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | Switzerland |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Switzerland |
PublicationTitle | Frontiers in Psychology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Front Psychol |
PublicationYear | 2012 |
Publisher | Frontiers Media SA Frontiers Research Foundation Frontiers Media S.A |
Publisher_xml | – name: Frontiers Media SA – name: Frontiers Research Foundation – name: Frontiers Media S.A |
References | 16504951 - Neuron. 2006 Mar 2;49(5):765-75 18482859 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Jun;12(6):209-10; author reply 210 19836292 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2009 Dec;13(12):517-23 20133167 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Dec;19(4):1069-78 15738960 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005 Mar;6(3):247-55 12214432 - Psychol Assess. 2002 Sep;14(3):253-62 11239442 - Neuron. 2001 Feb;29(2):537-45 19812332 - J Neurosci. 2009 Oct 7;29(40):12574-83 15557557 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004 Nov 30;101(48):16709-10 16286932 - Nat Neurosci. 2005 Dec;8(12):1704-11 12003856 - Cereb Cortex. 2002 Jun;12(6):565-74 15928095 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jun 7;102(23):8351-6 17237774 - Nat Neurosci. 2007 Feb;10(2):257-61 15808493 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Apr;9(4):159-62; discussion 162-4 19828331 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Jun;19(2):674-81 8039375 - Cognition. 1994 Apr-Jun;50(1-3):7-15 18178511 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Feb;12(2):54-8 16116449 - Nat Neurosci. 2005 Sep;8(9):1220-7 15501919 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004 Nov 9;101(45):16075-80 12649484 - Science. 2003 Mar 21;299(5614):1898-902 17259960 - Nat Neurosci. 2007 Feb;10(2):140-1 18835129 - Neural Netw. 2008 Nov;21(9):1247-60 11583523 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2001 Nov;80(3):201-24 10377356 - J Neurosci. 1999 Jul 1;19(13):5473-81 10526345 - Nat Neurosci. 1999 Nov;2(11):1032-7 10731224 - Cereb Cortex. 2000 Mar;10(3):295-307 20005133 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Mar;19(1):352-63 10519925 - Cognition. 1999 Oct 26;72(3):269-304 16197997 - Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(2):239-71 7566086 - Nature. 1995 Sep 28;377(6547):336-8 16484496 - Science. 2006 Feb 17;311(5763):1005-7 11448261 - JAMA. 2001 Jul 11;286(2):141-4 15653813 - Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2004 Sep;3(3):159-72 19228971 - J Neurosci. 2009 Feb 18;29(7):2188-92 11316014 - Psychol Bull. 2001 Mar;127(2):267-86 17122317 - J Neurophysiol. 2007 Feb;97(2):1621-32 16899731 - J Neurosci. 2006 Aug 9;26(32):8360-7 9036851 - Science. 1997 Feb 28;275(5304):1293-5 19423820 - Science. 2009 May 8;324(5928):759-64 11691967 - Science. 2001 Nov 2;294(5544):980-2 17362380 - Psychol Sci. 2007 Jan;18(1):64-71 16339445 - Science. 2005 Dec 9;310(5754):1680-3 17403642 - Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007 May 29;362(1481):857-75 20888231 - Curr Biol. 2010 Oct 26;20(20):1823-9 11697867 - Conscious Cogn. 2001 Sep;10(3):294-340 15588617 - Neuroimage. 2005 Jan 1;24(1):253-9 12403997 - Nat Neurosci. 2002 Nov;5(11):1103-4; author reply 1104 16880132 - Neuron. 2006 Aug 3;51(3):381-90 |
References_xml | – reference: 11583523 - J Exp Child Psychol. 2001 Nov;80(3):201-24 – reference: 9036851 - Science. 1997 Feb 28;275(5304):1293-5 – reference: 19828331 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Jun;19(2):674-81 – reference: 11691967 - Science. 2001 Nov 2;294(5544):980-2 – reference: 15653813 - Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2004 Sep;3(3):159-72 – reference: 10731224 - Cereb Cortex. 2000 Mar;10(3):295-307 – reference: 11316014 - Psychol Bull. 2001 Mar;127(2):267-86 – reference: 19836292 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2009 Dec;13(12):517-23 – reference: 16484496 - Science. 2006 Feb 17;311(5763):1005-7 – reference: 16504951 - Neuron. 2006 Mar 2;49(5):765-75 – reference: 16286932 - Nat Neurosci. 2005 Dec;8(12):1704-11 – reference: 10526345 - Nat Neurosci. 1999 Nov;2(11):1032-7 – reference: 11697867 - Conscious Cogn. 2001 Sep;10(3):294-340 – reference: 18482859 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Jun;12(6):209-10; author reply 210 – reference: 15928095 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005 Jun 7;102(23):8351-6 – reference: 16880132 - Neuron. 2006 Aug 3;51(3):381-90 – reference: 12403997 - Nat Neurosci. 2002 Nov;5(11):1103-4; author reply 1104 – reference: 12214432 - Psychol Assess. 2002 Sep;14(3):253-62 – reference: 11239442 - Neuron. 2001 Feb;29(2):537-45 – reference: 19423820 - Science. 2009 May 8;324(5928):759-64 – reference: 16116449 - Nat Neurosci. 2005 Sep;8(9):1220-7 – reference: 15501919 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004 Nov 9;101(45):16075-80 – reference: 10519925 - Cognition. 1999 Oct 26;72(3):269-304 – reference: 17259960 - Nat Neurosci. 2007 Feb;10(2):140-1 – reference: 15588617 - Neuroimage. 2005 Jan 1;24(1):253-9 – reference: 18178511 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2008 Feb;12(2):54-8 – reference: 17403642 - Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007 May 29;362(1481):857-75 – reference: 10377356 - J Neurosci. 1999 Jul 1;19(13):5473-81 – reference: 11448261 - JAMA. 2001 Jul 11;286(2):141-4 – reference: 15738960 - Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005 Mar;6(3):247-55 – reference: 7566086 - Nature. 1995 Sep 28;377(6547):336-8 – reference: 20133167 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Dec;19(4):1069-78 – reference: 19228971 - J Neurosci. 2009 Feb 18;29(7):2188-92 – reference: 17122317 - J Neurophysiol. 2007 Feb;97(2):1621-32 – reference: 17237774 - Nat Neurosci. 2007 Feb;10(2):257-61 – reference: 18835129 - Neural Netw. 2008 Nov;21(9):1247-60 – reference: 16899731 - J Neurosci. 2006 Aug 9;26(32):8360-7 – reference: 8039375 - Cognition. 1994 Apr-Jun;50(1-3):7-15 – reference: 15808493 - Trends Cogn Sci. 2005 Apr;9(4):159-62; discussion 162-4 – reference: 19812332 - J Neurosci. 2009 Oct 7;29(40):12574-83 – reference: 12003856 - Cereb Cortex. 2002 Jun;12(6):565-74 – reference: 17362380 - Psychol Sci. 2007 Jan;18(1):64-71 – reference: 20005133 - Conscious Cogn. 2010 Mar;19(1):352-63 – reference: 16197997 - Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(2):239-71 – reference: 15557557 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004 Nov 30;101(48):16709-10 – reference: 16339445 - Science. 2005 Dec 9;310(5754):1680-3 – reference: 20888231 - Curr Biol. 2010 Oct 26;20(20):1823-9 – reference: 12649484 - Science. 2003 Mar 21;299(5614):1898-902 |
SSID | ssj0000402002 |
Score | 2.0530546 |
Snippet | To what extent can people choose advantageously without knowing why they are making those choices? This hotly debated question has capitalized on the Iowa... |
SourceID | doaj pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref nii |
SourceType | Open Website Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 50 |
SubjectTerms | BF1-990 confidence Consciousness decision making Post-decision wagering Psychology risk aversion |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV09T8NADLUQUxfENwGKbmBhiJo0ySVhA0RVMTBRqdspcXxQCUIFBan_HvuSVi1CsLB0aBP18ny130tebYBzyiwiodiqdODHRNrPkApfIzG51RQnlXNb3OvhKL4bJ-OVUV_iCWvaAzfA9UqbBERZmhesRfIq5oKe5Mh1U8KLqesEymVsRUy5HCyySKw78lySVVjes9P3-aNYuVyDTvmb_Uodcu36ubrUk8lPTPO7YXKlAg22YauljuqqWfIObFC9C51lBpvvwSUHXYlfUL1aJaZxVXw298PUpFYs9H1Wv1zzWO2rqh2u47-4eVT7MBrcPtwM_XY4go9Jomd-n5iZRLoIiSlGUGBFzCQisY0EGHANj_KqLDEvMss5SdrecRy49Jc2zCyfitEBbPL30hGoFGUIetQPUQdxVAY5os7Ikk3QUlaGHvQWUBlsO4fLAItnwwpCwDUOXCPgGgeuBxfLM6ZN14xfjr0W9JfHSb9r9wbvAtPuAvPXLvCgy7Hj1clryDlIpJZYuBpGoz1Qi6ga_v3IQ5GiJkbb5JyzwjTqJx4cNkFeroSZUyrjbzxI18K_ttT1T-rJk2vRHfUzzUTo-D-u7QQ6glZz3-cUNmdvH9RlJjQrz9ym_wK05QJv priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals |
Title | The Role of Risk Aversion in Non-Conscious Decision Making |
URI | https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1870021790882151296 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22375133 https://www.proquest.com/docview/925717325 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC3286853 https://doaj.org/article/bf50ee879a1849d483959c6582004c74 |
Volume | 3 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
journalDatabaseRights | – providerCode: PRVAFT databaseName: Open Access Digital Library customDbUrl: eissn: 1664-1078 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002 issn: 1664-1078 databaseCode: KQ8 dateStart: 20100101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: http://grweb.coalliance.org/oadl/oadl.html providerName: Colorado Alliance of Research Libraries – providerCode: PRVAON databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals customDbUrl: eissn: 1664-1078 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002 issn: 1664-1078 databaseCode: DOA dateStart: 20100101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://www.doaj.org/ providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals – providerCode: PRVBFR databaseName: Free Medical Journals customDbUrl: eissn: 1664-1078 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002 issn: 1664-1078 databaseCode: DIK dateStart: 20100101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: http://www.freemedicaljournals.com providerName: Flying Publisher – providerCode: PRVFQY databaseName: GFMER Free Medical Journals customDbUrl: eissn: 1664-1078 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002 issn: 1664-1078 databaseCode: GX1 dateStart: 0 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: http://www.gfmer.ch/Medical_journals/Free_medical.php providerName: Geneva Foundation for Medical Education and Research – providerCode: PRVHPJ databaseName: ROAD: Directory of Open Access Scholarly Resources (ISSN International Center) customDbUrl: eissn: 1664-1078 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002 issn: 1664-1078 databaseCode: M~E dateStart: 20100101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://road.issn.org providerName: ISSN International Centre – providerCode: PRVAQN databaseName: PubMed Central customDbUrl: eissn: 1664-1078 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002 issn: 1664-1078 databaseCode: RPM dateStart: 20100101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/ providerName: National Library of Medicine – providerCode: PRVFZP databaseName: Scholars Portal Journals: Open Access customDbUrl: eissn: 1664-1078 dateEnd: 20250131 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0000402002 issn: 1664-1078 databaseCode: M48 dateStart: 20101201 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: http://journals.scholarsportal.info providerName: Scholars Portal |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwhV1Lb9RADLZQe-kFUV4bSqs5cOEQNs9JUgmhtlCtkNpDxUp7GyVeT0lZJe3utmL_PfYkXbqowCWHJCNZn8f254ljA7yj3CISSlmVDvyESPs5UulrJCa3mpJ06qotzvVonHydpJPfv0f3AC4eTe1kntR4Pvvw82b1iQ3-o2ScHG-H9nqxupQqLdd7UxL4bY5Lkezxs57sO78sqZKU8wz-tnAjNrkW_hxxmrp-jH3-WUT5ICqdPoOnPZ1UR53-d-HJVfkcdtZebfUCDnkjqIt2Rqq16qJe_FBHd90Zmaobdd42vszsRCmFVZ_7gTvqzM2oegnj0y_fTkZ-PzDBxzTVSz8iZiuxLkNi2hGUOCVmF7GUkgQYcFyPi2lVYVHmlv2UtMJj3TAdqGyYW16K8SvYatqGBqAylMHocRSiDpK4CgpEnZMlm6KlvAo9GN5DZbDvJi5DLWaGswoB1zhwjYBrHLgevF-vuO46afzj3WNBf_2e9MB2N9r5pelNylQ2DYjyrCg5Sy2mCVO9tEBmVGL4mCUe7LPuWDq5huyXJP2Ssq6O5WgP1L1WDduUfCgpG2K0TcF-LMziKPXgdafktSTMpjIZieNBtqH-DVE3nzT1d9e2O45yzeTozX_E2oMdAaI75nkLW8v5Le0z8VlWB-7A4MDt6l-RWPzx |
linkProvider | Scholars Portal |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+Role+of+Risk+Aversion+in+Non-Conscious+Decision+Making&rft.jtitle=Frontiers+in+Psychology&rft.au=Shuo+eWang&rft.au=Ian+eKrajbich&rft.au=Ralph+eAdolphs&rft.au=Naotsugu+eTsuchiya&rft.date=2012-01-01&rft.pub=Frontiers+Media+SA&rft.eissn=1664-1078&rft.volume=3&rft_id=info:doi/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2012.00050 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1664-1078&client=summon |