The sensory origins of human position sense
Key points Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm. Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibratio...
Saved in:
Published in | The Journal of physiology Vol. 594; no. 4; pp. 1037 - 1049 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
England
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
15.02.2016
John Wiley and Sons Inc |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0022-3751 1469-7793 1469-7793 |
DOI | 10.1113/JP271498 |
Cover
Abstract | Key points
Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm.
Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch.
Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks.
It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing.
Human limb position sense can be measured in two ways: in a blindfolded matching task, position of one limb is indicated with the other limb. Alternatively, position of a limb, hidden from view, is indicated with a pointer, moved by pressing a lever. These experiments examined the sensory basis of position sense measured in these two ways. Position errors were measured in 14 subjects after elbow flexors or extensors had been conditioned with a half‐maximum voluntary contraction. In agreement with previous studies, in the matching trials, position errors were distributed according to a pattern consistent with the action of muscle spindles as the position sensors. In the pointing trials, all errors lay in the direction of extension of the true position of the hidden arm and their distribution was inconsistent with influences arising in muscle spindles. Vibration of elbow muscles produced an illusion of muscle lengthening during a matching task, while during the pointing task no illusion was present. Finally, the matching–pointing error difference was preserved, even when one arm was loaded with a weight or skin over the elbow was stretched. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense. One is signalled by muscle spindles, indicating position of one part of the body relative to another. A second provides information about the position of the body in extrapersonal space and here we hypothesise that exteroceptors, including vision, touch and hearing, acting via a central map of the body, provide the spatial information.
Key points
Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm.
Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch.
Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks.
It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm. Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch. Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing.KEY POINTSPosition sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm. Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch. Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing.Human limb position sense can be measured in two ways: in a blindfolded matching task, position of one limb is indicated with the other limb. Alternatively, position of a limb, hidden from view, is indicated with a pointer, moved by pressing a lever. These experiments examined the sensory basis of position sense measured in these two ways. Position errors were measured in 14 subjects after elbow flexors or extensors had been conditioned with a half-maximum voluntary contraction. In agreement with previous studies, in the matching trials, position errors were distributed according to a pattern consistent with the action of muscle spindles as the position sensors. In the pointing trials, all errors lay in the direction of extension of the true position of the hidden arm and their distribution was inconsistent with influences arising in muscle spindles. Vibration of elbow muscles produced an illusion of muscle lengthening during a matching task, while during the pointing task no illusion was present. Finally, the matching-pointing error difference was preserved, even when one arm was loaded with a weight or skin over the elbow was stretched. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense. One is signalled by muscle spindles, indicating position of one part of the body relative to another. A second provides information about the position of the body in extrapersonal space and here we hypothesise that exteroceptors, including vision, touch and hearing, acting via a central map of the body, provide the spatial information.ABSTRACTHuman limb position sense can be measured in two ways: in a blindfolded matching task, position of one limb is indicated with the other limb. Alternatively, position of a limb, hidden from view, is indicated with a pointer, moved by pressing a lever. These experiments examined the sensory basis of position sense measured in these two ways. Position errors were measured in 14 subjects after elbow flexors or extensors had been conditioned with a half-maximum voluntary contraction. In agreement with previous studies, in the matching trials, position errors were distributed according to a pattern consistent with the action of muscle spindles as the position sensors. In the pointing trials, all errors lay in the direction of extension of the true position of the hidden arm and their distribution was inconsistent with influences arising in muscle spindles. Vibration of elbow muscles produced an illusion of muscle lengthening during a matching task, while during the pointing task no illusion was present. Finally, the matching-pointing error difference was preserved, even when one arm was loaded with a weight or skin over the elbow was stretched. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense. One is signalled by muscle spindles, indicating position of one part of the body relative to another. A second provides information about the position of the body in extrapersonal space and here we hypothesise that exteroceptors, including vision, touch and hearing, acting via a central map of the body, provide the spatial information. Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm. Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch. Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing. Human limb position sense can be measured in two ways: in a blindfolded matching task, position of one limb is indicated with the other limb. Alternatively, position of a limb, hidden from view, is indicated with a pointer, moved by pressing a lever. These experiments examined the sensory basis of position sense measured in these two ways. Position errors were measured in 14 subjects after elbow flexors or extensors had been conditioned with a half-maximum voluntary contraction. In agreement with previous studies, in the matching trials, position errors were distributed according to a pattern consistent with the action of muscle spindles as the position sensors. In the pointing trials, all errors lay in the direction of extension of the true position of the hidden arm and their distribution was inconsistent with influences arising in muscle spindles. Vibration of elbow muscles produced an illusion of muscle lengthening during a matching task, while during the pointing task no illusion was present. Finally, the matching-pointing error difference was preserved, even when one arm was loaded with a weight or skin over the elbow was stretched. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense. One is signalled by muscle spindles, indicating position of one part of the body relative to another. A second provides information about the position of the body in extrapersonal space and here we hypothesise that exteroceptors, including vision, touch and hearing, acting via a central map of the body, provide the spatial information. Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm. Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch. Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing. Key points Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm. Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch. Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing. Human limb position sense can be measured in two ways: in a blindfolded matching task, position of one limb is indicated with the other limb. Alternatively, position of a limb, hidden from view, is indicated with a pointer, moved by pressing a lever. These experiments examined the sensory basis of position sense measured in these two ways. Position errors were measured in 14 subjects after elbow flexors or extensors had been conditioned with a half‐maximum voluntary contraction. In agreement with previous studies, in the matching trials, position errors were distributed according to a pattern consistent with the action of muscle spindles as the position sensors. In the pointing trials, all errors lay in the direction of extension of the true position of the hidden arm and their distribution was inconsistent with influences arising in muscle spindles. Vibration of elbow muscles produced an illusion of muscle lengthening during a matching task, while during the pointing task no illusion was present. Finally, the matching–pointing error difference was preserved, even when one arm was loaded with a weight or skin over the elbow was stretched. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense. One is signalled by muscle spindles, indicating position of one part of the body relative to another. A second provides information about the position of the body in extrapersonal space and here we hypothesise that exteroceptors, including vision, touch and hearing, acting via a central map of the body, provide the spatial information. Key points Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm. Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch. Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing. Key points Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm. Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch. Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing. Human limb position sense can be measured in two ways: in a blindfolded matching task, position of one limb is indicated with the other limb. Alternatively, position of a limb, hidden from view, is indicated with a pointer, moved by pressing a lever. These experiments examined the sensory basis of position sense measured in these two ways. Position errors were measured in 14 subjects after elbow flexors or extensors had been conditioned with a half-maximum voluntary contraction. In agreement with previous studies, in the matching trials, position errors were distributed according to a pattern consistent with the action of muscle spindles as the position sensors. In the pointing trials, all errors lay in the direction of extension of the true position of the hidden arm and their distribution was inconsistent with influences arising in muscle spindles. Vibration of elbow muscles produced an illusion of muscle lengthening during a matching task, while during the pointing task no illusion was present. Finally, the matching-pointing error difference was preserved, even when one arm was loaded with a weight or skin over the elbow was stretched. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense. One is signalled by muscle spindles, indicating position of one part of the body relative to another. A second provides information about the position of the body in extrapersonal space and here we hypothesise that exteroceptors, including vision, touch and hearing, acting via a central map of the body, provide the spatial information. Key points Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived position of an unseen arm. Effects on position sense tested were: elbow muscle conditioning with a voluntary contraction, muscle vibration, loading the arm and elbow skin stretch. Conditioning contractions and vibration produced errors in a matching task, consistent with the action of muscle spindles as position sensors. Position errors in a pointing task were not consistent with the action of muscle spindles. Loading the arm or skin stretch had no effect in either matching or pointing tasks. It is proposed that there are two kinds of position sense: (i) indicating positions of different body parts relative to one another, using signals from muscle spindles; and (ii) indicating position of the body in extrapersonal space, using signals from exteroceptors, vision, touch and hearing. |
Author | Giummarra, M. J. Allen, T. J. Proske, U. Tsay, A. J. |
AuthorAffiliation | 1 School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences Monash University Clayton Vic 3800 Australia 2 Caulfield Pain Management and Research Centre Caulfield Hospital Caulfield Vic 3162 Australia 3 Accident Research Centre Monash Injury Research Institute Clayton Vic 3800 Australia 4 Department of Physiology Monash University Clayton Vic 3800 Australia |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 4 Department of Physiology Monash University Clayton Vic 3800 Australia – name: 1 School of Psychological Sciences and Monash Institute of Cognitive and Clinical Neurosciences Monash University Clayton Vic 3800 Australia – name: 2 Caulfield Pain Management and Research Centre Caulfield Hospital Caulfield Vic 3162 Australia – name: 3 Accident Research Centre Monash Injury Research Institute Clayton Vic 3800 Australia |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: A. J. surname: Tsay fullname: Tsay, A. J. organization: Caulfield Hospital – sequence: 2 givenname: M. J. surname: Giummarra fullname: Giummarra, M. J. organization: Caulfield Hospital – sequence: 3 givenname: T. J. surname: Allen fullname: Allen, T. J. organization: Monash Injury Research Institute – sequence: 4 givenname: U. surname: Proske fullname: Proske, U. organization: Monash University |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26537335$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp1kVlLAzEQx4NUbD3ATyALvgiymmRydF8EKZ4I-lCfQzZN25RtUpOu0m_v2sMLfZmBmd_859pFLR-8ReiQ4DNCCJzfP1FJWNHdQh3CRJFLWUALdTCmNAfJSRvtpjTBmAAuih3UpoKDBOAddNof2yxZn0JcZCG6kfMpC8NsXE-1z2YhubkLfknYfbQ91FWyB2u_h56vr_q92_zh8eaud_mQG45Zt7FsgMGyrtFMMIk5E5SXIDUVhgEfWDnUtIkbYUutQfOilBZLQaQBVhINe-hipTury6kdGOvnUVdqFt1Ux4UK2qmfGe_GahReFZMcqMCNwMlaIIaX2qa5mrpkbFVpb0OdFJGCc1lwKhv0-Bc6CXX0zXpLCnPglDfU0feJPkfZ3PGro4khpWiHnwjB6uNFavOiBj37hRo31x9XbnZx1V8F-argzVV28a-w6t8_iQK68A71Np3T |
CODEN | JPHYA7 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_016_4680_y crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_018_5460_7 crossref_primary_10_1109_TNSRE_2022_3161948 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jbmt_2024_03_006 crossref_primary_10_1109_TMRB_2024_3407532 crossref_primary_10_1152_jn_00713_2017 crossref_primary_10_1002_hbm_24739 crossref_primary_10_1249_MSS_0000000000002444 crossref_primary_10_56061_fbujohs_1334050 crossref_primary_10_1155_2018_5353242 crossref_primary_10_1186_s12984_021_00870_y crossref_primary_10_3233_BMR_230219 crossref_primary_10_1113_EP089826 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_021_06207_4 crossref_primary_10_3233_RNN_160660 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_023_06689_4 crossref_primary_10_1113_JP271854 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heliyon_2023_e18884 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_019_05634_8 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_020_05999_1 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jpain_2016_03_005 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2020_105016 crossref_primary_10_1113_JP280230 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_018_5280_9 crossref_primary_10_3390_app112210522 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_reach_2019_100030 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15767 crossref_primary_10_3233_BMR_230129 crossref_primary_10_1007_s00221_023_06582_0 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_14050 crossref_primary_10_13066_kspm_2024_19_4_103 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_lssr_2021_11_003 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0297074 crossref_primary_10_1113_JP272998 |
Cites_doi | 10.1093/brain/95.4.705 10.1007/s00221-014-4121-8 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.192336 10.1007/BF00253639 10.1007/s00221-014-3842-z 10.1139/y88-073 10.1016/0301-0082(93)90032-N 10.1007/s00221-012-3281-7 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.009 10.1152/jn.1985.54.6.1529 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.103093 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.028 10.1038/35784 10.1152/jn.1988.59.4.1220 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017067 10.1007/s00221-014-4088-5 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008184 10.1007/s002210000380 10.1152/jn.1991.66.2.602 10.1007/s00221-008-1561-z 10.1007/s00221-015-4334-5 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.125161 10.1152/jn.00494.2009 10.1073/pnas.1003483107 10.1111/joa.12289 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.028 10.1016/0167-9457(89)90016-X 10.1007/s00221-014-3911-3 10.1007/s00221-015-4242-8 10.1152/jn.00191.2005 10.1152/physrev.00048.2011 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.269365 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.259127 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.187732 10.1113/jphysiol.1960.sp006520 10.1093/brain/111.2.281 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021733 10.1007/s00221-015-4220-1 10.1007/s00221-005-0174-z 10.1007/s00221-005-0280-y 10.1152/japplphysiol.91365.2008 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.092619 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.115097 10.1093/brain/96.2.269 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2015 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology © 2015 The Physiological Society 2015 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology © 2015 The Physiological Society. Journal compilation © 2016 The Physiological Society |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2015 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology © 2015 The Physiological Society – notice: 2015 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology © 2015 The Physiological Society. – notice: Journal compilation © 2016 The Physiological Society |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QP 7QR 7TK 7TS 8FD FR3 P64 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1113/JP271498 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Physical Education Index Technology Research Database Engineering Research Database Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Technology Research Database Chemoreception Abstracts Engineering Research Database Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Physical Education Index Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE - Academic MEDLINE CrossRef Technology Research Database |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology |
DocumentTitleAlternate | A. J. Tsay and others |
EISSN | 1469-7793 |
EndPage | 1049 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC4753260 3952576351 26537335 10_1113_JP271498 TJP6938 |
Genre | article Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- -DZ -~X .3N .GA 05W 0R~ 0YM 10A 123 18M 1OB 1OC 24P 29L 2WC 33P 36B 3SF 4.4 50Y 50Z 51W 51X 52M 52N 52O 52P 52R 52S 52T 52U 52V 52W 52X 53G 5GY 5HH 5LA 5RE 5VS 66C 702 7PT 8-0 8-1 8-3 8-4 8-5 8UM 930 A01 A03 AAESR AAEVG AAFWJ AAHHS AAHQN AAIPD AAMNL AANLZ AAONW AASGY AAXRX AAYCA AAZKR ABCQN ABCUV ABEML ABITZ ABIVO ABJNI ABOCM ABPPZ ABPVW ABQWH ABXGK ACAHQ ACCFJ ACCZN ACFBH ACGFO ACGFS ACGOF ACIWK ACMXC ACNCT ACPOU ACPRK ACSCC ACXBN ACXQS ADBBV ADBTR ADEOM ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADOZA ADXAS ADZMN AEEZP AEGXH AEIGN AEIMD AEQDE AEUQT AEUYR AFBPY AFEBI AFFPM AFGKR AFPWT AFWVQ AFZJQ AHBTC AI. AIACR AIAGR AITYG AIURR AIWBW AJBDE ALAGY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQN ALVPJ AMBMR AMYDB AOIJS ATUGU AZBYB AZVAB BAFTC BAWUL BFHJK BHBCM BMXJE BROTX BRXPI BY8 C45 CS3 D-6 D-7 D-E D-F DCZOG DIK DPXWK DR2 DRFUL DRMAN DRSTM E3Z EBS EJD EMOBN EX3 F00 F01 F04 F5P FIJ FUBAC G-S G.N GODZA GX1 H.X HGLYW HZI HZ~ IHE IX1 J0M K48 KBYEO LATKE LC2 LC3 LEEKS LH4 LITHE LOXES LP6 LP7 LUTES LW6 LYRES MEWTI MK4 MRFUL MRMAN MRSTM MSFUL MSMAN MSSTM MXFUL MXMAN MXSTM N04 N05 N9A NF~ O66 O9- OIG OK1 OVD P2P P2W P2X P2Z P4B P4D Q.N Q11 QB0 R.K ROL RPM RX1 SUPJJ TEORI TLM TN5 TR2 UB1 UPT V8K VH1 W8F W8V W99 WBKPD WH7 WIH WIJ WIK WIN WNSPC WOHZO WOQ WOW WQJ WRC WXI WXSBR WYISQ XG1 YBU YHG YKV YQT YSK YZZ ZZTAW ~IA ~WT AAYXX AEYWJ AGHNM AGYGG CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QP 7QR 7TK 7TS 8FD FR3 P64 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c5048-c54d03e48ca4647054625b37a26c435de7fa2470c6ebaa3a59b7e07617c34b1a3 |
IEDL.DBID | DR2 |
ISSN | 0022-3751 1469-7793 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 14:13:38 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 09:17:16 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 11:59:33 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 06:02:40 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 04:29:09 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:50:24 EDT 2025 Wed Jan 22 16:37:47 EST 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 4 |
Language | English |
License | http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/termsAndConditions#vor 2015 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology © 2015 The Physiological Society. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c5048-c54d03e48ca4647054625b37a26c435de7fa2470c6ebaa3a59b7e07617c34b1a3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/4753260 |
PMID | 26537335 |
PQID | 1765053525 |
PQPubID | 1086388 |
PageCount | 13 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_4753260 proquest_miscellaneous_1765579527 proquest_journals_1765053525 pubmed_primary_26537335 crossref_primary_10_1113_JP271498 crossref_citationtrail_10_1113_JP271498 wiley_primary_10_1113_JP271498_TJP6938 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 15 February 2016 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2016-02-15 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 02 year: 2016 text: 15 February 2016 day: 15 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | England |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: England – name: London – name: Hoboken |
PublicationTitle | The Journal of physiology |
PublicationTitleAlternate | J Physiol |
PublicationYear | 2016 |
Publisher | Wiley Subscription Services, Inc John Wiley and Sons Inc |
Publisher_xml | – name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc – name: John Wiley and Sons Inc |
References | 2009; 47 1973; 96 2012; 223 2010; 107 2007; 580 2010; 103 1960; 153 2010; 588 1988; 59 1993; 41 1989; 8 2006; 571 2006; 170 2000; 133 2015; 227 2014; 232 2014; 592 2006; 576 1998; 391 2012; 92 1989; 76 2009; 192 1991; 66 2006; 44 2015; 233 2005; 568 1967; 189 1988; 399 1988; 66 1988; 111 1996; 496 2013; 591 2005; 94 1972; 95 1985; 54 2012; 22 2006; 168 2009; 106 e_1_2_6_32_1 e_1_2_6_10_1 e_1_2_6_31_1 e_1_2_6_30_1 e_1_2_6_19_1 e_1_2_6_13_1 e_1_2_6_36_1 e_1_2_6_14_1 e_1_2_6_35_1 e_1_2_6_11_1 e_1_2_6_34_1 e_1_2_6_12_1 e_1_2_6_33_1 e_1_2_6_17_1 e_1_2_6_18_1 e_1_2_6_39_1 e_1_2_6_15_1 e_1_2_6_38_1 e_1_2_6_16_1 e_1_2_6_37_1 Gregory JE (e_1_2_6_20_1) 1991; 66 e_1_2_6_42_1 e_1_2_6_43_1 e_1_2_6_21_1 e_1_2_6_41_1 e_1_2_6_40_1 e_1_2_6_9_1 e_1_2_6_8_1 e_1_2_6_5_1 e_1_2_6_4_1 e_1_2_6_7_1 e_1_2_6_6_1 e_1_2_6_25_1 e_1_2_6_24_1 e_1_2_6_3_1 e_1_2_6_23_1 e_1_2_6_2_1 e_1_2_6_22_1 e_1_2_6_29_1 e_1_2_6_44_1 e_1_2_6_28_1 e_1_2_6_45_1 e_1_2_6_27_1 e_1_2_6_26_1 24099798 - J Physiol. 2013 Dec 1;591(23):6103-14 24671653 - Exp Brain Res. 2014 Jul;232(7):2197-206 26048159 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 Sep;233(9):2635-43 2753103 - Exp Brain Res. 1989;76(1):213-22 24468725 - Exp Brain Res. 2014 Apr;232(4):1259-66 3048612 - Can J Physiol Pharmacol. 1988 Apr;66(4):430-8 25665873 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 May;233(5):1463-70 25757958 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 Jun;233(6):1689-701 24665096 - J Physiol. 2014 Jun 15;592(12):2679-94 25973697 - J Anat. 2015 Aug;227(2):178-83 23073629 - Physiol Rev. 2012 Oct;92(4):1651-97 8930850 - J Physiol. 1996 Nov 1;496 ( Pt 3):857-71 1774588 - J Neurophysiol. 1991 Aug;66(2):602-12 16311728 - Exp Brain Res. 2006 Jan;168(1-2):307-11 20547672 - J Physiol. 2010 Aug 1;588(Pt 15):2789-800 18762203 - Neuropsychologia. 2009 Jan;47(1):204-11 22789996 - Curr Biol. 2012 Jul 10;22(13):R516-7 16873408 - J Physiol. 2006 Oct 15;576(Pt 2):445-55 16439427 - J Physiol. 2006 Mar 15;571(Pt 3):703-10 4715185 - Brain. 1973 Jun;96(2):269-76 8140258 - Prog Neurobiol. 1993 Dec;41(6):705-21 25200179 - Exp Brain Res. 2014 Nov;232(11):3397-412 4265060 - Brain. 1972;95(4):705-48 15917323 - J Neurophysiol. 2005 Sep;94(3):1699-706 25294499 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 Feb;233(2):375-83 16109730 - J Physiol. 2005 Nov 1;568(Pt 3):1035-46 4087047 - J Neurophysiol. 1985 Dec;54(6):1529-40 16328298 - Exp Brain Res. 2006 Mar;170(1):30-8 10958524 - Exp Brain Res. 2000 Aug;133(3):340-8 6040161 - J Physiol. 1967 Apr;189(3):545-50 18787812 - Exp Brain Res. 2009 Jan;192(1):113-20 9486643 - Nature. 1998 Feb 19;391(6669):756 19118155 - J Appl Physiol (1985). 2009 Mar;106(3):950-8 3404468 - J Physiol. 1988 May;399:49-61 23111431 - Exp Brain Res. 2012 Dec;223(4):541-51 26876442 - J Physiol. 2016 Feb 15;594(4):801-2 17255167 - J Physiol. 2007 Apr 15;580(Pt. 2):423-34 19864441 - J Neurophysiol. 2010 Jan;103(1):164-71 3378137 - Brain. 1988 Apr;111 ( Pt 2):281-97 20547858 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010 Jun 29;107(26):11727-32 20194123 - J Physiol. 2010 Apr 15;588(Pt 8):1369-77 16750227 - Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(12):2430-6 3373276 - J Neurophysiol. 1988 Apr;59(4):1220-30 |
References_xml | – volume: 233 start-page: 1689 year: 2015 end-page: 1701 article-title: Untangling visual and proprioceptive contributions to hand localisation over time publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 189 start-page: 545 year: 1967 end-page: 550 article-title: Fusimotor stimulation and the dynamic sensitivity of the secondary ending of the muscle spindle publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 8 start-page: 177 year: 1989 end-page: 193 article-title: Elbow position sense in man: Contrasting results in matching and pointing publication-title: Hum Movement Sci – volume: 133 start-page: 340 year: 2000 end-page: 348 article-title: Effects of body orientation, load and vibration on sensing position and movement at the human elbow joint publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 576 start-page: 445 year: 2006 end-page: 455 article-title: Position sense at the human forearm in the horizontal plane during loading and vibration of elbow muscles publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 233 start-page: 2635 year: 2015 end-page: 2634 article-title: Position sense at the human forearm after conditioning elbow muscles with isometric contractions publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 106 start-page: 950 year: 2009 end-page: 958 article-title: Signals of motor command bias joint position sense in the presence of feedback from proprioceptors publication-title: J Appl Physiol – volume: 192 start-page: 113 year: 2009 end-page: 120 article-title: Illusions of forearm displacement during vibration of elbow muscles in humans publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 47 start-page: 204 year: 2009 end-page: 211 article-title: The rubber hand illusion in action publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 591 start-page: 6103 year: 2013 end-page: 6114 article-title: The contribution of motor commands to position sense differs between elbow and wrist publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 571 start-page: 703 year: 2006 end-page: 710 article-title: Motor commands contribute to human position sense publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 107 start-page: 11727 year: 2010 end-page: 11732 article-title: An implicit body representation underlying human position sense publication-title: Proc Natl Acad Sci USA – volume: 588 start-page: 2789 year: 2010 end-page: 2800 article-title: The illusion of changed position and movement from vibrating one arm is altered by vision or movement of the other arm publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 111 start-page: 281 year: 1988 end-page: 297 article-title: Some proprioceptive influences on the perceptual representation of body shape and orientation publication-title: Brain – volume: 232 start-page: 3397 year: 2014 end-page: 3412 article-title: Muscle thixotropy as a tool in the study of proprioception publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 568 start-page: 1035 year: 2005 end-page: 1046 article-title: Muscle spindle signals combine with the sense of effort to indicate limb position publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 153 start-page: 188 year: 1960 end-page: 198 article-title: Properties of cutaneous touch receptors in cat publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 41 start-page: 705 year: 1993 end-page: 721 article-title: Thixotropy in skeletal muscle and in muscle spindles: A review publication-title: Prog Neurobiol – volume: 232 start-page: 1259 year: 2014 end-page: 1266 article-title: Combining proprioception and touch to compute spatial information publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 223 start-page: 541 year: 2012 end-page: 551 article-title: Interaction between vibration‐evoked proprioceptive illusions and mirror‐evoked visual illusions in an arm‐matching task publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 76 start-page: 213 year: 1989 end-page: 222 article-title: Alteration of proprioceptive messages induced by tendon vibration in man: a microneurographic study publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 96 start-page: 269 year: 1973 end-page: 276 article-title: Position sense after surgical disconnexion of the cerebral hemispheres in man publication-title: Brain – volume: 399 start-page: 49 year: 1988 end-page: 61 article-title: Position sense at the proximal interphalangeal joint of the human index finger publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 95 start-page: 705 year: 1972 end-page: 748 article-title: The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents publication-title: Brain – volume: 103 start-page: 164 year: 2010 end-page: 171 article-title: Where is your arm? Variations in proprioception across space and tasks publication-title: J Neurophysiol – volume: 54 start-page: 1529 year: 1985 end-page: 1540 article-title: Role of intramuscular receptors in the awareness of limb position publication-title: J Neurophysiol – volume: 44 start-page: 2430 year: 2006 end-page: 2436 article-title: Dissociating body representations in healthy individuals: Differential effects of a kinaesthetic illusion on perception and action publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 66 start-page: 602 year: 1991 end-page: 612 article-title: Two kinds of resting discharge in cat muscle spindles publication-title: J Neurophysiol – volume: 22 start-page: R516 year: 2012 end-page: R517 article-title: Action sounds recalibrate perceived tactile distance publication-title: Curr Biol – volume: 94 start-page: 1699 year: 2005 end-page: 1706 article-title: Cutaneous receptors contribute to kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee publication-title: J Neurophysiol – volume: 92 start-page: 1651 year: 2012 end-page: 1697 article-title: The proprioceptive senses: Their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force publication-title: Physiol Rev – volume: 233 start-page: 375 year: 2015 end-page: 383 article-title: Illusory movements induced by tendon vibration in right‐ and left‐handed people publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 592 start-page: 2679 year: 2014 end-page: 2694 article-title: Limb position sense, proprioceptive drift and muscle thixotropy at the human elbow joint publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 232 start-page: 2197 year: 2014 end-page: 2206 article-title: Proprioceptive illusions created by vibration of one arm are altered by vibrating the other arm publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 588 start-page: 1369 year: 2010 end-page: 1377 article-title: The effect of fatigue from exercise on human limb position sense publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 168 start-page: 307 year: 2006 end-page: 311 article-title: Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 66 start-page: 430 year: 1988 end-page: 438 article-title: Proprioceptors and their contribution to somatosensory mapping: Complex messages require complex processing publication-title: Can J Physiol Pharmcol – volume: 227 start-page: 178 year: 2015 end-page: 183 article-title: The role of muscle proprioceptors in human limb position sense: a hypothesis publication-title: J Anat – volume: 59 start-page: 1220 year: 1988 end-page: 1230 article-title: Aftereffects in the responses of cat muscle spindles and errors of limb position sense in man publication-title: J Neurophysiol – volume: 580 start-page: 423 year: 2007 end-page: 434 article-title: Effects of muscle conditioning on position sense at the human forearm during loading or fatigue of elbow flexors and the role of the sense of effort publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 496 start-page: 857 year: 1996 end-page: 871 article-title: Movement illusions evoked by ensemble cutaneous input from the dorsum of the hand publication-title: J Physiol – volume: 233 start-page: 1463 year: 2015 end-page: 1470 article-title: Kinaesthetic mirror illusion and spatial congruence publication-title: Exp Brain Res – volume: 391 start-page: 756 year: 1998 end-page: 756 article-title: Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see publication-title: Nature – volume: 170 start-page: 30 year: 2006 end-page: 38 article-title: Effect of muscle fatigue on the sense of limb position and movement publication-title: Exp Brain Res – ident: e_1_2_6_17_1 doi: 10.1093/brain/95.4.705 – ident: e_1_2_6_38_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4121-8 – ident: e_1_2_6_23_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.192336 – ident: e_1_2_6_35_1 doi: 10.1007/BF00253639 – ident: e_1_2_6_9_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3842-z – ident: e_1_2_6_29_1 doi: 10.1139/y88-073 – ident: e_1_2_6_33_1 doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(93)90032-N – ident: e_1_2_6_41_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3281-7 – ident: e_1_2_6_25_1 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.009 – ident: e_1_2_6_10_1 doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.54.6.1529 – ident: e_1_2_6_15_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.103093 – ident: e_1_2_6_24_1 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.028 – ident: e_1_2_6_7_1 doi: 10.1038/35784 – ident: e_1_2_6_19_1 doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.59.4.1220 – ident: e_1_2_6_13_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1988.sp017067 – ident: e_1_2_6_34_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4088-5 – ident: e_1_2_6_8_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008184 – ident: e_1_2_6_18_1 doi: 10.1007/s002210000380 – volume: 66 start-page: 602 year: 1991 ident: e_1_2_6_20_1 article-title: Two kinds of resting discharge in cat muscle spindles publication-title: J Neurophysiol doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.2.602 – ident: e_1_2_6_44_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1561-z – ident: e_1_2_6_39_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4334-5 – ident: e_1_2_6_2_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.125161 – ident: e_1_2_6_14_1 doi: 10.1152/jn.00494.2009 – ident: e_1_2_6_27_1 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003483107 – ident: e_1_2_6_31_1 doi: 10.1111/joa.12289 – ident: e_1_2_6_37_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.028 – ident: e_1_2_6_42_1 doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(89)90016-X – ident: e_1_2_6_21_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3911-3 – ident: e_1_2_6_6_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4242-8 – ident: e_1_2_6_12_1 doi: 10.1152/jn.00191.2005 – ident: e_1_2_6_32_1 doi: 10.1152/physrev.00048.2011 – ident: e_1_2_6_40_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.269365 – ident: e_1_2_6_43_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.259127 – ident: e_1_2_6_3_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.187732 – ident: e_1_2_6_22_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1960.sp006520 – ident: e_1_2_6_26_1 doi: 10.1093/brain/111.2.281 – ident: e_1_2_6_11_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021733 – ident: e_1_2_6_30_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4220-1 – ident: e_1_2_6_4_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0174-z – ident: e_1_2_6_16_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0280-y – ident: e_1_2_6_36_1 doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91365.2008 – ident: e_1_2_6_45_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.092619 – ident: e_1_2_6_5_1 doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.115097 – ident: e_1_2_6_28_1 doi: 10.1093/brain/96.2.269 – reference: 18762203 - Neuropsychologia. 2009 Jan;47(1):204-11 – reference: 19864441 - J Neurophysiol. 2010 Jan;103(1):164-71 – reference: 8140258 - Prog Neurobiol. 1993 Dec;41(6):705-21 – reference: 23073629 - Physiol Rev. 2012 Oct;92(4):1651-97 – reference: 16873408 - J Physiol. 2006 Oct 15;576(Pt 2):445-55 – reference: 24665096 - J Physiol. 2014 Jun 15;592(12):2679-94 – reference: 25200179 - Exp Brain Res. 2014 Nov;232(11):3397-412 – reference: 16439427 - J Physiol. 2006 Mar 15;571(Pt 3):703-10 – reference: 10958524 - Exp Brain Res. 2000 Aug;133(3):340-8 – reference: 17255167 - J Physiol. 2007 Apr 15;580(Pt. 2):423-34 – reference: 8930850 - J Physiol. 1996 Nov 1;496 ( Pt 3):857-71 – reference: 24099798 - J Physiol. 2013 Dec 1;591(23):6103-14 – reference: 2753103 - Exp Brain Res. 1989;76(1):213-22 – reference: 16109730 - J Physiol. 2005 Nov 1;568(Pt 3):1035-46 – reference: 3373276 - J Neurophysiol. 1988 Apr;59(4):1220-30 – reference: 19118155 - J Appl Physiol (1985). 2009 Mar;106(3):950-8 – reference: 20547858 - Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010 Jun 29;107(26):11727-32 – reference: 25294499 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 Feb;233(2):375-83 – reference: 3404468 - J Physiol. 1988 May;399:49-61 – reference: 9486643 - Nature. 1998 Feb 19;391(6669):756 – reference: 25665873 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 May;233(5):1463-70 – reference: 4265060 - Brain. 1972;95(4):705-48 – reference: 3048612 - Can J Physiol Pharmacol. 1988 Apr;66(4):430-8 – reference: 4715185 - Brain. 1973 Jun;96(2):269-76 – reference: 22789996 - Curr Biol. 2012 Jul 10;22(13):R516-7 – reference: 26048159 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 Sep;233(9):2635-43 – reference: 20194123 - J Physiol. 2010 Apr 15;588(Pt 8):1369-77 – reference: 23111431 - Exp Brain Res. 2012 Dec;223(4):541-51 – reference: 15917323 - J Neurophysiol. 2005 Sep;94(3):1699-706 – reference: 20547672 - J Physiol. 2010 Aug 1;588(Pt 15):2789-800 – reference: 4087047 - J Neurophysiol. 1985 Dec;54(6):1529-40 – reference: 1774588 - J Neurophysiol. 1991 Aug;66(2):602-12 – reference: 25973697 - J Anat. 2015 Aug;227(2):178-83 – reference: 26876442 - J Physiol. 2016 Feb 15;594(4):801-2 – reference: 16750227 - Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(12):2430-6 – reference: 25757958 - Exp Brain Res. 2015 Jun;233(6):1689-701 – reference: 3378137 - Brain. 1988 Apr;111 ( Pt 2):281-97 – reference: 18787812 - Exp Brain Res. 2009 Jan;192(1):113-20 – reference: 16311728 - Exp Brain Res. 2006 Jan;168(1-2):307-11 – reference: 24671653 - Exp Brain Res. 2014 Jul;232(7):2197-206 – reference: 16328298 - Exp Brain Res. 2006 Mar;170(1):30-8 – reference: 24468725 - Exp Brain Res. 2014 Apr;232(4):1259-66 – reference: 6040161 - J Physiol. 1967 Apr;189(3):545-50 |
SSID | ssj0013099 |
Score | 2.3837028 |
Snippet | Key points
Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the... Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the perceived... Key points Position sense at the human forearm can be measured in blindfolded subjects by matching positions of the arms or by a subject pointing to the... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref wiley |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 1037 |
SubjectTerms | Body Image Cognitive and Behavioural Neuroscience Elbow Female Forearm - innervation Forearm - physiology Humans Male Movement Muscle Contraction Muscle, Skeletal - innervation Muscle, Skeletal - physiology Neuroscience – behavioural/systems/cognitive Personal Space Psychomotor Performance Research Paper Sensation Sensory Neuroscience Skin Sports injuries Young Adult |
Title | The sensory origins of human position sense |
URI | https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1113%2FJP271498 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26537335 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1765053525 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1765579527 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC4753260 |
Volume | 594 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
journalDatabaseRights | – providerCode: PRVBFR databaseName: Free Medical Journals customDbUrl: eissn: 1469-7793 dateEnd: 20241001 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0013099 issn: 0022-3751 databaseCode: DIK dateStart: 18780101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: http://www.freemedicaljournals.com providerName: Flying Publisher – providerCode: PRVFQY databaseName: GFMER Free Medical Journals customDbUrl: eissn: 1469-7793 dateEnd: 20241001 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0013099 issn: 0022-3751 databaseCode: GX1 dateStart: 18780101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: http://www.gfmer.ch/Medical_journals/Free_medical.php providerName: Geneva Foundation for Medical Education and Research – providerCode: PRVAQN databaseName: PubMed Central customDbUrl: eissn: 1469-7793 dateEnd: 20241001 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0013099 issn: 0022-3751 databaseCode: RPM dateStart: 18780101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/ providerName: National Library of Medicine – providerCode: PRVWIB databaseName: Wiley Online Library - Core collection (SURFmarket) issn: 0022-3751 databaseCode: DR2 dateStart: 19970101 customDbUrl: isFulltext: true eissn: 1469-7793 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: false ssIdentifier: ssj0013099 providerName: Wiley-Blackwell |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1bS8MwFA6yJ1-8zct0SgSZT51rkzTt4xDHGChDNhj4UJI0RVFb2eVh_npP0ovOKYgvLbQnvSQ9zfclJ99B6CIIXA2gTTkikR0gKDJ2pAzhZygTGgANksyq7d_e-f0xHUzYpIiqNGthcn2IasDNeIb9XxsHF7LIQuIasYHB0OMA7806X5cwO0N7731OIHTCsBIK58wtdGeh6FVZcLUnWoOX61GSX9Gr7X562-ihfPA86uS5vZjLtnr_pun4vzfbQVsFKsXd_DPaRRs63UP1bgqM_HWJW9jGidoB-DoyQRp4Buw3my5xnldrhrME22x_uAwCsxZ6H417N6PrvlOkXHAUA1-GLY07RNNACepTDngO-JEkXHi-AmAVa54ID44rX0shiGCh5NoMhXBFqHQFOUC1NEv1EcKcqpAFsSAEUJpRCYsBWSSwI2ECHCppoMuy-iNV6JGbtBgvUc5LSFTWQwOdV5ZvuQbHDzbNsgWjwgtnkcsBfxr9GgaXqE6D_5hJEZHqbJHbMB4yjzfQYd7g1U08nxFOCJTmK59CZWC0uVfPpE-PVqObAg0EqthALdvSvz53NBoM_ZAEx381PEGbgNls4LjLmqg2ny70KeCiuTyzHvABJMoFvA |
linkProvider | Wiley-Blackwell |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1bS8MwFD54edAX75d5jSD6NF2bpmnxSdQxpxsiE_YglCRNUdRO3Pagv96T9KLzAuJLC81JmyY5zfclp18AdoPA0QjaVFUksoYERcZVKUP8GMrEC5AGSWbV9lttv3HjNbusOwZHxb8wmT5EOeFmPMN-r42Dmwnp3MuN2kDzyuWI74NxmDTLc8YrT6_djyWEWhiWUuGcObnyLOY9LHKOjkXfAOb3OMnP-NUOQPVZuC2KnsWdPBwMB_JAvX1Rdfznu83BTA5MyXHWk-ZhTKcLsHicIil_eiV7xIaK2jn4RTBxGqSPBLj38kqyrbX6pJcQu-EfKeLArIVegpv6WeekUc13Xagqhu6MRy-uUe0FSmC1coR0SJEk5cL1FWKrWPNEuHhd-VoKQQULJddmNoQr6klH0GWYSHupXgXCPRWyIBaUIlAzQmExgosETzRMkEYlFdgv6j9SuSS52RnjMcqoCY2KeqjATmn5nMlw_GCzUTRhlDtiP3I4QlAjYcPwFmUyupBZFxGp7g0zG8ZD5vIKrGQtXj7E9RnllGJuPtIXSgMjzz2akt7fWZluD5kgssUK7Nmm_rXcUad55Yc0WPur4TZMNTqty-jyvH2xDtMI4WwcucM2YGLwMtSbCJMGcsu6wzt91wnY |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1JS8QwFH64gHhxX8Y1guhpdNokTXsUddBxYRAFwUNJ0hRF7QzOzEF_vS_pouMC4qWF5qVNk7zm-5LXLwDbYegZBG26LlPVQIKikrpSEX4MVcpCpEGKO7X9i8vg5Ia1bvltEVVp_4XJ9SGqCTfrGe57bR28m6SFk1uxgVbbFwjvw1EYZwHe1QKiK_9jBaERRZVSuOBeITyLeffLnMND0Td8-T1M8jN8deNPcxruypLnYSePe4O-2tNvX0Qd__dqMzBVwFJykPejWRgx2RzMH2RIyZ9fyQ5xgaJuBn4ebJQG6SH97by8knxjrR7ppMRt90fKKDBnYRbgpnl8fXhSL_ZcqGuOzoxHljSoYaGWLGACAR0SJEWF9AONyCoxIpU-XteBUVJSySMljJ0LEZoy5Um6CGNZJzPLQATTEQ8TSSnCNCsTliC0SPFEoxRJVFqD3bL6Y10Iktt9MZ7inJjQuKyHGmxVlt1chOMHm7WyBePCDXuxJxCAWgEbjreoktGB7KqIzExnkNtwEXFf1GApb_DqIX7AqaAUc4uhrlAZWHHu4ZTs4d6JdDPkgcgVa7DjWvrXcsfXrXYQ0XDlr4abMNE-asbnp5dnqzCJ-M0FkXt8Dcb6LwOzjhiprzacM7wDVwQIhw |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+sensory+origins+of+human+position+sense&rft.jtitle=The+Journal+of+physiology&rft.au=Tsay%2C+A+J&rft.au=Giummarra%2C+M+J&rft.au=Allen%2C+T+J&rft.au=Proske%2C+U&rft.date=2016-02-15&rft.issn=1469-7793&rft.eissn=1469-7793&rft.volume=594&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=1037&rft_id=info:doi/10.1113%2FJP271498&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0022-3751&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0022-3751&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0022-3751&client=summon |