Beyond “fake news”: Analytic thinking and the detection of false and hyperpartisan news headlines
Why is misleading partisan content believed and shared? An influential account posits that political partisanship pervasively biases reasoning, such that engaging in analytic thinking exacerbates motivated reasoning and, in turn, the acceptance of hyperpartisan content. Alternatively, it may be that...
Saved in:
Published in | Judgment and decision making Vol. 16; no. 2; pp. 484 - 504 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
Tallahassee
Society for Judgment and Decision Making
01.03.2021
Cambridge University Press |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1930-2975 1930-2975 |
DOI | 10.1017/S1930297500008640 |
Cover
Abstract | Why is misleading partisan content believed and shared? An influential account posits that political partisanship pervasively biases reasoning, such that engaging in analytic thinking exacerbates motivated reasoning and, in turn, the acceptance of hyperpartisan content. Alternatively, it may be that susceptibility to hyperpartisan content is explained by a
lack
of reasoning. Across two studies using different participant pools (total N = 1,973 Americans), we had participants assess true, false, and hyperpartisan news headlines taken from social media. We found no evidence that analytic thinking was associated with judging politically consistent hyperpartisan or false headlines to be accurate and unbiased. Instead, analytic thinking was, in most cases, associated with an increased tendency to distinguish true headlines from both false and hyperpartisan headlines (and was never associated with decreased discernment). These results suggest that reasoning typically helps people differentiate between low and high quality political news, rather than facilitate belief in misleading content. Because social media play an important role in the dissemination of misinformation, we also investigated willingness to share headlines on social media. We found a similar pattern whereby analytic thinking was not generally associated with increased willingness to share hyperpartisan or false headlines. Together, these results suggest a positive role for reasoning in resisting misinformation. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Why is misleading partisan content believed and shared? An influential account posits that political partisanship pervasively biases reasoning, such that engaging in analytic thinking exacerbates motivated reasoning and, in turn, the acceptance of hyperpartisan content. Alternatively, it may be that susceptibility to hyperpartisan content is explained by a lack of reasoning. Across two studies using different participant pools(total N = 1,973 Americans), we had participants assess true, false, and hyperpartisan news headlines taken from social media. We found no evidence that analytic thinking was associated with judging politically consistent hyperpartisan or false headlines to be accurate and unbiased. Instead, analytic thinking was, in most cases, associated with an increased tendency to distinguish true headlines from both false and hyperpartisan headlines (and was never associated with decreased discernment).These results suggest that reasoning typically helps people differentiate between low and high qual-ity political news, rather than facilitate belief in misleading content. Because social media play an important role in the dissemination of misinformation, we also investigated willingness to share headlines on social media. We found a similar pattern whereby analytic thinking was not generally associated with increased willingness to share hyperpartisan or false headlines. Together, these results suggest a positive role for reasoning in resisting misinformation. Why is misleading partisan content believed and shared? An influential account posits that political partisanship pervasively biases reasoning, such that engaging in analytic thinking exacerbates motivated reasoning and, in turn, the acceptance of hyperpartisan content. Alternatively, it may be that susceptibility to hyperpartisan content is explained by a lack of reasoning. Across two studies using different participant pools (total N = 1,973 Americans), we had participants assess true, false, and hyperpartisan news headlines taken from social media. We found no evidence that analytic thinking was associated with judging politically consistent hyperpartisan or false headlines to be accurate and unbiased. Instead, analytic thinking was, in most cases, associated with an increased tendency to distinguish true headlines from both false and hyperpartisan headlines (and was never associated with decreased discernment). These results suggest that reasoning typically helps people differentiate between low and high quality political news, rather than facilitate belief in misleading content. Because social media play an important role in the dissemination of misinformation, we also investigated willingness to share headlines on social media. We found a similar pattern whereby analytic thinking was not generally associated with increased willingness to share hyperpartisan or false headlines. Together, these results suggest a positive role for reasoning in resisting misinformation. Why is misleading partisan content believed and shared? An influential account posits that political partisanship pervasively biases reasoning, such that engaging in analytic thinking exacerbates motivated reasoning and, in turn, the acceptance of hyperpartisan content. Alternatively, it may be that susceptibility to hyperpartisan content is explained by a lack of reasoning. Across two studies using different participant pools (total N = 1,973 Americans), we had participants assess true, false, and hyperpartisan news headlines taken from social media. We found no evidence that analytic thinking was associated with judging politically consistent hyperpartisan or false headlines to be accurate and unbiased. Instead, analytic thinking was, in most cases, associated with an increased tendency to distinguish true headlines from both false and hyperpartisan headlines (and was never associated with decreased discernment). These results suggest that reasoning typically helps people differentiate between low and high quality political news, rather than facilitate belief in misleading content. Because social media play an important role in the dissemination of misinformation, we also investigated willingness to share headlines on social media. We found a similar pattern whereby analytic thinking was not generally associated with increased willingness to share hyperpartisan or false headlines. Together, these results suggest a positive role for reasoning in resisting misinformation. Keywords: dual-process theory, fake news, misinformation, news media, partisanship Why is misleading partisan content believed and shared? An influential account posits that political partisanship pervasively biases reasoning, such that engaging in analytic thinking exacerbates motivated reasoning and, in turn, the acceptance of hyperpartisan content. Alternatively, it may be that susceptibility to hyperpartisan content is explained by a lack of reasoning. Across two studies using different participant pools (total N = 1,973 Americans), we had participants assess true, false, and hyperpartisan news headlines taken from social media. We found no evidence that analytic thinking was associated with judging politically consistent hyperpartisan or false headlines to be accurate and unbiased. Instead, analytic thinking was, in most cases, associated with an increased tendency to distinguish true headlines from both false and hyperpartisan headlines (and was never associated with decreased discernment). These results suggest that reasoning typically helps people differentiate between low and high quality political news, rather than facilitate belief in misleading content. Because social media play an important role in the dissemination of misinformation, we also investigated willingness to share headlines on social media. We found a similar pattern whereby analytic thinking was not generally associated with increased willingness to share hyperpartisan or false headlines. Together, these results suggest a positive role for reasoning in resisting misinformation. |
Audience | Academic |
Author | Ross, Robert M. Rand, David G. Pennycook, Gordon |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Robert M. orcidid: 0000-0001-8711-1675 surname: Ross fullname: Ross, Robert M. – sequence: 2 givenname: David G. orcidid: 0000-0001-8975-2783 surname: Rand fullname: Rand, David G. – sequence: 3 givenname: Gordon orcidid: 0000-0003-1344-6143 surname: Pennycook fullname: Pennycook, Gordon |
BookMark | eNp9kcuOFCEUhokZE2dGH8BdJa57BKqAwl078TLJJC7UNTlFHbrpqYYWmJjezYPoy82TSHUb7xESIIf__7j8Z-QkxICEPGX0glGmnr9nuqVcK0Fr62VHH5DTubSYaye_rB-Rs5w3lAquqTol-BL3MYzN_d0XBzfYBPyc7---vmiWAaZ98bYpax9ufFg1UGVljc2IBW3xMTTRNQ6mjIet9X6HaQep-AzhwGnWCOPkA-bH5OFB-OT7fE4-vn714fLt4vrdm6vL5fXCdj0vi9bpTgKjSKXotO2ttlJpqcY6dLRKAKFHpFyKwQlnlegoZx046xyXLW_PydWRO0bYmF3yW0h7E8GbQyGmlZkvaCc0YkBRGYPVQ2XjAKxX4EC1WjONdqysZ0fWLsVPt5iL2cTbVH8lGy6okq3umPypWkGF-uBiSWC3PluzlJJJzWmrq-riH6raR9x6W5N0vtZ_M7CjwaaYc0L34zGMmjlw81fg1aP-8FhfYA6qHuan_zi_AfeXsJ0 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_024_02086_1 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_021_01219_5 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2022_105312 crossref_primary_10_5993_AJHB_48_3_18 crossref_primary_10_1080_19393555_2024_2378747 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41562_024_02067_4 crossref_primary_10_1093_poq_nfae044 crossref_primary_10_1177_14614448221130955 crossref_primary_10_1057_s41599_023_02282_w crossref_primary_10_1002_acp_4130 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_023_47939_3 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2023_1165039 crossref_primary_10_3390_bs14040278 crossref_primary_10_1002_acp_70029 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_ins_2024_121097 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0316989 crossref_primary_10_1177_10776990221110113 crossref_primary_10_1108_OIR_02_2024_0111 crossref_primary_10_1017_jdm_2024_36 crossref_primary_10_1177_00936502231206419 crossref_primary_10_3390_jintelligence12110109 crossref_primary_10_1177_17506980221094516 crossref_primary_10_5964_ejop_13657 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13421_024_01564_1 crossref_primary_10_3389_fcomm_2024_1472631 crossref_primary_10_1002_pra2_790 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_chb_2022_107295 crossref_primary_10_3390_socsci13110608 crossref_primary_10_1109_ACCESS_2022_3213681 crossref_primary_10_1177_00222437241281873 crossref_primary_10_1080_10410236_2023_2206177 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_heliyon_2025_e42255 crossref_primary_10_1177_15554589241259407 crossref_primary_10_3390_bs13110896 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_paid_2024_112747 crossref_primary_10_1177_01461672231154886 crossref_primary_10_1177_00187208211072642 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_025_90427_z crossref_primary_10_1177_00219096241287710 crossref_primary_10_1089_cyber_2024_0407 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jesp_2022_104404 crossref_primary_10_3758_s13423_023_02321_2 crossref_primary_10_1057_s41599_023_01813_9 |
Cites_doi | 10.1037/xge0000673 10.1287/mnsc.2019.3478 10.1007/s12144-019-0130-x 10.1371/journal.pone.0228882 10.1126/science.aap9559 10.1038/srep00335 10.1038/s41467-020-20043-0 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00009 10.1257/089533005775196732 10.17645/mac.v9i1.3519 10.1177/0146167218783192 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.01.003 10.1017/S1930297500001819 10.1177/2053168018822174 10.1111/jopy.12476 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199230167.001.0001 10.1007/s10683-011-9273-9 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.011 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.05.001 10.37016/mr-2020-009 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.01.003 10.1038/s41586-021-03344-2 10.1037/xge0000974 10.1177/1745691612460685 10.1111/pops.12407 10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480 10.4324/9780429326769-23 10.1177/0963721415604610 10.37016/mr-2020-51 10.1038/s41562-020-0833-x 10.1038/s41562-017-0213-3 10.1037/h0101832 10.1017/S1930297500007695 10.3758/s13428-017-0963-x 10.1073/pnas.1806781116 10.1017/S1930297500007622 10.1016/j.tics.2021.02.007 10.1017/bpp.2016.2 10.2139/ssrn.2973067 10.31234/osf.io/98zwc 10.7208/chicago/9780226771199.001.0001 10.1037/xge0000729 10.1080/17467586.2016.1198042 10.1126/sciadv.aau4586 10.1126/science.aao2998 10.1111/pops.12681 10.1037/a0025391 10.1126/science.aau2706 10.1177/0956797620939054 10.1037/xge0000465 10.1111/pops.12055 10.1080/10584609.2019.1663322 10.1017/S1930297500005271 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | COPYRIGHT 2021 Society for Judgment and Decision Making 2021. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: COPYRIGHT 2021 Society for Judgment and Decision Making – notice: 2021. This work is published under http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/ (the “License”). Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION 3V. 7WY 7WZ 7XB 87Z 8FK 8FL 8G5 ABUWG AFKRA AZQEC BENPR BEZIV CCPQU DWQXO FRNLG F~G GNUQQ GUQSH K60 K6~ L.- M0C M2O MBDVC PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PKEHL PQBIZ PQBZA PQEST PQQKQ PQUKI Q9U DOA |
DOI | 10.1017/S1930297500008640 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef ProQuest Central (Corporate) ABI/INFORM Collection ABI/INFORM Global (PDF only) ProQuest Central (purchase pre-March 2016) ABI/INFORM Collection ProQuest Central (Alumni) (purchase pre-March 2016) ABI/INFORM Collection (Alumni) ProQuest Research Library ProQuest Central (Alumni) ProQuest Central UK/Ireland ProQuest Central Essentials - QC ProQuest Central Business Premium Collection ProQuest One Community College ProQuest Central Korea Business Premium Collection (Alumni) ABI/INFORM Global (Corporate) ProQuest Central Student ProQuest Research Library ProQuest Business Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Business Collection ABI/INFORM Professional Advanced ABI/INFORM Global Research Library Research Library (Corporate) Proquest Central Premium ProQuest One Academic (New) Publicly Available Content Database ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest One Business ProQuest One Business (Alumni) ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition (DO NOT USE) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest Central Basic DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef Publicly Available Content Database ABI/INFORM Global (Corporate) ProQuest Business Collection (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Business Research Library Prep ProQuest Central Student ProQuest One Academic Middle East (New) ProQuest Central Essentials ProQuest Central (Alumni Edition) ProQuest One Community College Research Library (Alumni Edition) ABI/INFORM Complete ProQuest Central ABI/INFORM Professional Advanced ProQuest Central Korea ProQuest Research Library ProQuest Central (New) ABI/INFORM Complete (Alumni Edition) Business Premium Collection ABI/INFORM Global ABI/INFORM Global (Alumni Edition) ProQuest Central Basic ProQuest One Academic Eastern Edition ProQuest Business Collection ProQuest One Academic UKI Edition ProQuest One Business (Alumni) ProQuest One Academic ProQuest One Academic (New) ProQuest Central (Alumni) Business Premium Collection (Alumni) |
DatabaseTitleList | Publicly Available Content Database CrossRef |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: DOA name: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals url: https://www.doaj.org/ sourceTypes: Open Website – sequence: 2 dbid: BENPR name: ProQuest Central url: http://www.proquest.com/pqcentral?accountid=15518 sourceTypes: Aggregation Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1930-2975 |
EndPage | 504 |
ExternalDocumentID | oai_doaj_org_article_5be5f5fbc9b640eba187afa739919ecd A661692039 10_1017_S1930297500008640 |
GeographicLocations | United States |
GeographicLocations_xml | – name: United States |
GroupedDBID | 09C 09D 0R~ 188 2UF 2WC 5VS 7WY 8FL 8G5 AAFWJ AASVR AAYXX ABDBF ABGDZ ABIVO ABJNI ABUWG ABXAU ABXHF ACAJB ACDLN ACDXY ACHQT ACUHS ADBBV ADKIL ADVJH AEBAK AFKRA AFKRZ AFMMW AFPKN AFZFC AGHGI AGTDA AHRGI AINHJ AKMAY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ATFKH AZQEC BCNDV BENPR BEZIV BPHCQ C1A CCPQU CDVRH CITATION CNMHZ DWQXO E3Z EAD EAP EBS EJD EPL ESX FRNLG GNUQQ GROUPED_ABI_INFORM_RESEARCH GROUPED_DOAJ GUQSH H13 IAO ICO IEA IPNFZ IPY IPYYG ITC JDM K60 K6~ KQ8 M0C M2O M~E OK1 P2P PHGZM PHGZT PIMPY PQBIZ PQBZA PQQKQ PROAC RCA RIG ROL TH9 TR2 TUS UZ2 PMFND 3V. 7XB 8FK AAXMD L.- MBDVC PKEHL PQEST PQUKI Q9U PUEGO |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c482t-3f946a10e06549c8c9c67967d79640482aea8ee0265bf5fc7540214afcff26323 |
IEDL.DBID | BENPR |
ISSN | 1930-2975 |
IngestDate | Wed Aug 27 01:28:10 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 22:38:02 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 17 21:49:41 EDT 2025 Tue Jun 10 20:47:25 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:53:37 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 00:18:42 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 2 |
Language | English |
License | http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0 |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c482t-3f946a10e06549c8c9c67967d79640482aea8ee0265bf5fc7540214afcff26323 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 |
ORCID | 0000-0003-1344-6143 0000-0001-8711-1675 0000-0001-8975-2783 |
OpenAccessLink | https://www.proquest.com/docview/2507639416?pq-origsite=%requestingapplication%&accountid=15518 |
PQID | 2507639416 |
PQPubID | 696407 |
PageCount | 21 |
ParticipantIDs | doaj_primary_oai_doaj_org_article_5be5f5fbc9b640eba187afa739919ecd proquest_journals_2507639416 gale_infotracmisc_A661692039 gale_infotracacademiconefile_A661692039 crossref_primary_10_1017_S1930297500008640 crossref_citationtrail_10_1017_S1930297500008640 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2021-03-00 20210301 2021-03-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2021-03-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 03 year: 2021 text: 2021-03-00 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | Tallahassee |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: Tallahassee |
PublicationTitle | Judgment and decision making |
PublicationYear | 2021 |
Publisher | Society for Judgment and Decision Making Cambridge University Press |
Publisher_xml | – name: Society for Judgment and Decision Making – name: Cambridge University Press |
References | S1930297500008640_rf48 S1930297500008640_rf49 Dias (S1930297500008640_rf9) 2020; 1 S1930297500008640_rf51 S1930297500008640_rf52 S1930297500008640_rf54 S1930297500008640_rf10 S1930297500008640_rf11 S1930297500008640_rf12 S1930297500008640_rf56 Stagnaro (S1930297500008640_rf55) 2018; 13 S1930297500008640_rf13 S1930297500008640_rf57 S1930297500008640_rf14 Kohlberg (S1930297500008640_rf29) 1969 S1930297500008640_rf58 Fisk (S1930297500008640_rf15) 1984 S1930297500008640_rf50 S1930297500008640_rf16 S1930297500008640_rf17 S1930297500008640_rf19 S1930297500008640_rf62 S1930297500008640_rf20 S1930297500008640_rf64 S1930297500008640_rf65 S1930297500008640_rf21 S1930297500008640_rf22 S1930297500008640_rf23 S1930297500008640_rf24 Piaget (S1930297500008640_rf53) 1932 S1930297500008640_rf60 S1930297500008640_rf61 Thomson (S1930297500008640_rf59) 2016; 11 S1930297500008640_rf3 S1930297500008640_rf2 S1930297500008640_rf1 S1930297500008640_rf7 S1930297500008640_rf26 S1930297500008640_rf27 S1930297500008640_rf5 S1930297500008640_rf4 Kahan (S1930297500008640_rf25) 2013; 8 S1930297500008640_rf8 S1930297500008640_rf30 S1930297500008640_rf31 S1930297500008640_rf32 S1930297500008640_rf33 Martel (S1930297500008640_rf35) 2020; 5 S1930297500008640_rf34 S1930297500008640_rf36 Webster (S1930297500008640_rf63) 1994; 67 S1930297500008640_rf37 Cokely (S1930297500008640_rf6) 2012; 7 S1930297500008640_rf38 S1930297500008640_rf39 Kahneman (S1930297500008640_rf28) 2011 S1930297500008640_rf40 S1930297500008640_rf41 S1930297500008640_rf42 S1930297500008640_rf43 S1930297500008640_rf44 S1930297500008640_rf45 S1930297500008640_rf46 S1930297500008640_rf47 Guess (S1930297500008640_rf18) 2020; 1 |
References_xml | – ident: S1930297500008640_rf4 doi: 10.1037/xge0000673 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf39 doi: 10.1287/mnsc.2019.3478 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf65 doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-0130-x – ident: S1930297500008640_rf37 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0228882 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf61 doi: 10.1126/science.aap9559 – volume: 1 start-page: 1 year: 2020 ident: S1930297500008640_rf18 article-title: “Fake news” may have limited effects on political participation beyond increasing beliefs in false claims. publication-title: Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review – ident: S1930297500008640_rf64 doi: 10.1038/srep00335 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf36 doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-20043-0 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf51 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00009 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf16 doi: 10.1257/089533005775196732 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf34 doi: 10.17645/mac.v9i1.3519 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf47 doi: 10.1177/0146167218783192 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf57 doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.01.003 – volume: 7 start-page: 25 year: 2012 ident: S1930297500008640_rf6 article-title: Measuring risk literacy: The Berlin Numeracy Test. publication-title: Judgment and Decision Making doi: 10.1017/S1930297500001819 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf7 doi: 10.1177/2053168018822174 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf50 doi: 10.1111/jopy.12476 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf10 doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199230167.001.0001 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf21 doi: 10.1007/s10683-011-9273-9 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf49 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.011 – volume-title: The moral judgment of the child year: 1932 ident: S1930297500008640_rf53 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf43 doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.05.001 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf13 doi: 10.37016/mr-2020-009 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf24 doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.01.003 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf41 doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-03344-2 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf58 doi: 10.1037/xge0000974 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf11 doi: 10.1177/1745691612460685 – volume: 5 year: 2020 ident: S1930297500008640_rf35 article-title: Reliance on emotion promotes belief in fake news. publication-title: Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications – ident: S1930297500008640_rf23 doi: 10.1111/pops.12407 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf31 doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf33 doi: 10.4324/9780429326769-23 – volume: 1 start-page: 1 year: 2020 ident: S1930297500008640_rf9 article-title: Emphasizing publishers does not effectively reduce susceptibility to misinformation on social media. publication-title: Harvard Kennedy School Misinformation Review – ident: S1930297500008640_rf42 doi: 10.1177/0963721415604610 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf45 doi: 10.37016/mr-2020-51 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf62 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf20 doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0833-x – volume-title: Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research year: 1969 ident: S1930297500008640_rf29 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf8 doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0213-3 – volume-title: Thinking, fast and slow year: 2011 ident: S1930297500008640_rf28 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf5 doi: 10.1037/h0101832 – volume-title: Social Cognition year: 1984 ident: S1930297500008640_rf15 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf52 – volume: 13 start-page: 260 year: 2018 ident: S1930297500008640_rf55 article-title: Performance on the Cognitive Reflection Test is stable across time. publication-title: Judgment and Decision Making doi: 10.1017/S1930297500007695 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf2 doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0963-x – ident: S1930297500008640_rf48 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1806781116 – volume: 11 start-page: 99 year: 2016 ident: S1930297500008640_rf59 article-title: Investigating an alternative form of the Cognitive Reflection Test. publication-title: Judgment and Decision Making doi: 10.1017/S1930297500007622 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf46 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.02.007 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf27 doi: 10.1017/bpp.2016.2 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf26 doi: 10.2139/ssrn.2973067 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf38 doi: 10.31234/osf.io/98zwc – ident: S1930297500008640_rf56 doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226771199.001.0001 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf1 doi: 10.1037/xge0000729 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf22 – volume: 67 start-page: 1049 year: 1994 ident: S1930297500008640_rf63 article-title: Individual differences in Need for Cognitive Closure. publication-title: Personality Processes and Individual Differences – ident: S1930297500008640_rf30 doi: 10.1080/17467586.2016.1198042 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf19 doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aau4586 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf32 doi: 10.1126/science.aao2998 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf60 doi: 10.1111/pops.12681 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf54 doi: 10.1037/a0025391 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf17 doi: 10.1126/science.aau2706 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf44 doi: 10.1177/0956797620939054 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf40 doi: 10.1037/xge0000465 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf14 doi: 10.1111/pops.12055 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf12 – ident: S1930297500008640_rf3 doi: 10.1080/10584609.2019.1663322 – volume: 8 start-page: 407 year: 2013 ident: S1930297500008640_rf25 article-title: Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection. publication-title: Judgment and Decision Making doi: 10.1017/S1930297500005271 |
RelatedPersons | Trump, Donald Clinton, Hillary Rodham |
RelatedPersons_xml | – fullname: Trump, Donald – fullname: Clinton, Hillary Rodham |
SSID | ssj0052907 |
Score | 2.4563239 |
Snippet | Why is misleading partisan content believed and shared? An influential account posits that political partisanship pervasively biases reasoning, such that... |
SourceID | doaj proquest gale crossref |
SourceType | Open Website Aggregation Database Enrichment Source Index Database |
StartPage | 484 |
SubjectTerms | Bias Clinton, Hillary Rodham Cognition & reasoning Disinformation dual-process theory fake news False information misinformation News media Partisanship partisanshipnakeywords Publishing industry Social media Social networks Trump, Donald |
SummonAdditionalLinks | – databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals dbid: DOA link: http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwrV1LS8QwEA7iyYv4xNVVchAEodimadN4U3ERQU8K3kKeLChV3Hrwtj9E_9z-EmfS7rKLqBeveZR0Mpl8SWa-IeSQAWgodGqSkhuXcOFCopkwSW6DCBVzjMco15vb8uqeXz8UD3OpvtAnrKUHbgV3UhhfhCIYK03JU290VgkdtICNNZPeOrS-sI1ND1OtDS6YjIHSgE7SBGNHp--ZSBaNhVgW4VKJtx5zO1Ik7v_JPMc9Z7BGVjuwSM_aQa6TJV9vkJWZzXrfJL6NQKGT8UfQj54iSJ6MP09pJBuBbrQZttkRqIZmgPao8010v6rpc6ABtM_HqiEcSF9fUB4jXcfvULDTDlHoaIvcDy7vLq6SLnNCYnnFmiQPkpc6Sz2GjkpbWWnxvkg4DDyFNcu015X3cP4qDAjXCsBtLOM62BCQwD3fJsv1c-13CAzKcINAKcic5z6tvLMhcwYfUIXN0x5Jp9JTtqMVx-wWT6r1HxPqm8B75HjW5aXl1Pit8TlOyawh0mHHAlAS1SmJ-ktJeuQIJ1ThooXBWd3FHsAvIv2VOgOUUkqW5rJH-gstYbHZxeqpSqhusY8UCAestARou_sfg90jKwwdZ6KjW58sN69vfh-QT2MOopJ_AWpU_qM priority: 102 providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals |
Title | Beyond “fake news”: Analytic thinking and the detection of false and hyperpartisan news headlines |
URI | https://www.proquest.com/docview/2507639416 https://doaj.org/article/5be5f5fbc9b640eba187afa739919ecd |
Volume | 16 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
journalDatabaseRights | – providerCode: PRVAEN databaseName: Cambridge Wholly Gold Open Access Journals customDbUrl: eissn: 1930-2975 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0052907 issn: 1930-2975 databaseCode: IPYYG dateStart: 20060701 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://www.cambridge.org providerName: Cambridge University Press – providerCode: PRVAFT databaseName: Open Access Digital Library customDbUrl: eissn: 1930-2975 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0052907 issn: 1930-2975 databaseCode: KQ8 dateStart: 20060101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: http://grweb.coalliance.org/oadl/oadl.html providerName: Colorado Alliance of Research Libraries – providerCode: PRVAON databaseName: DOAJ Directory of Open Access Journals customDbUrl: eissn: 1930-2975 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0052907 issn: 1930-2975 databaseCode: DOA dateStart: 20060101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://www.doaj.org/ providerName: Directory of Open Access Journals – providerCode: PRVEBS databaseName: EBSCOhost Academic Search Ultimate customDbUrl: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?authtype=ip,shib&custid=s3936755&profile=ehost&defaultdb=asn eissn: 1930-2975 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0052907 issn: 1930-2975 databaseCode: ABDBF dateStart: 20110801 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://search.ebscohost.com/direct.asp?db=asn providerName: EBSCOhost – providerCode: PRVHPJ databaseName: ROAD: Directory of Open Access Scholarly Resources customDbUrl: eissn: 1930-2975 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0052907 issn: 1930-2975 databaseCode: M~E dateStart: 20060101 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://road.issn.org providerName: ISSN International Centre – providerCode: PRVPQU databaseName: ProQuest Central customDbUrl: http://www.proquest.com/pqcentral?accountid=15518 eissn: 1930-2975 dateEnd: 20221130 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0052907 issn: 1930-2975 databaseCode: BENPR dateStart: 20060701 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://www.proquest.com/central providerName: ProQuest |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwfV1LaxRBEC7i5pKLxBeuiaEPgiA0zvQ8eloQSSQxCi5BDCSnpp8JKLPr7njwlh-ify6_xKqemdUg5jj9GHq6q6q_6a76CuCZQNBQmczyurSel9JHboS0vHBRxkZ4UaYo14-z-vi0_HBWnW3AbIyFIbfK0SYmQ-3njs7IX-JWjaqgED-8WXzjlDWKblfHFBpmSK3gXyeKsTuwKSir8gQ2Dw5nJ59G21wJlQKoEbVknGJKx3tOIpGmQipLMKqm05C_dqpE6P8_s532oqNtuDuASLbfr_o92Ajtfdha27IfDyD0kSns-upnNF8CI_B8ffXrFUskJNiNdZd91gRmsBmiQOZDl9yyWjaPLKJUhlR1iT-qywUJ2Mq06T0M7bcndLp6CKdHh5_fHvMhowJ3ZSM6XkRV1ibPAoWUKtc45egcSXoKSEVdFiaYJgT8L6tsrKKTiOdEXproYiRi9-IRTNp5Gx4DDsqWlgBUVEVZhKwJ3sXcW7pYla7IppCNs6fdQDdOWS--6t6vTOp_JnwKL9ZdFj3Xxm2ND2hJ1g2JJjsVzJcXetA6XdlQ4XdYpyx2CdbkjTTRSERluQrOT-E5LagmZcbBOTPEJOAnEi2W3kf0UiuRFWoKuzdaohK6m9WjSOjBCKz0H5F9cnv1DmwJcpVJrm27MOmW38NTxDqd3RsEeC-dFeDT-5Pz83e_AdJi_bM |
linkProvider | ProQuest |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwtV1JTxRBFH5BOMjFgEscRayDxsSkY3f1WibEgEIGgYkxkHAra5VE0zPMNCHc-CH6V_wx_BLfq-4eJUZuXGtLLW_5quotAC84goZcxToqMm2jrLQ-UrzUUWp86StueRa8XA9GxfAo-3icHy_Ar94Xhswqe5kYBLUdG3ojf4OqGllBIH54NzmNKGsU_a72KTRUl1rBboQQY51jx567OMcr3Gxj9wOe90vOd7YP3w-jLstAZLKKN1HqRVaoJHbkZilMZYSht5XSkpMm0jdXTlXO4V0l1z73pkSMw5NMeeM9BTtPcdw7sISwI0WuWtraHn363OuCnIvgsI0oKY7Ih7X_V6Wg1VRIZQG2FfT68pdmDAkE_qcmgu7bWYF7HWhlmy2VrcKCq-_D8lx2XjwA13rCsKvLH159c4zA-tXlz7csBD3Bbqw5abM0MIXNEHUy65pgBlazsWceucCFqhO8GE8nRNAzVYdxGOoLS2h49hCObmVvH8FiPa7dY8BJ6UwTYPMizVIXV84an1hNH7mlSeMBxP3uSdOFN6csG99la8dWyn82fACv510mbWyPmxpv0ZHMG1JY7lAwnn6VHZfLXLsc16GN0NjFaZVUpfKqRBSYCGfsAF7RgUoSHjg5ozofCFwiheGSm4iWCsHjVAxg7VpLZHpzvbonCdkJnZn8wyJPbq5-DneHhwf7cn93tPcUljmZ6QSzujVYbKZn7hnirEavd8TM4Mtt889va2o21Q |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Beyond+%22fake+news%22%3A+Analytic+thinking+and+the+detection+of+false+and+hyperpartisan+news+headlines&rft.jtitle=Judgment+and+decision+making&rft.au=Ross%2C+Robert+M&rft.au=Rand%2C+David+G&rft.au=Pennycook%2C+Gordon&rft.date=2021-03-01&rft.pub=Society+for+Judgment+and+Decision+Making&rft.issn=1930-2975&rft.eissn=1930-2975&rft.volume=16&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=484&rft_id=info:doi/10.1017%2FS1930297500008640&rft.externalDocID=A661692039 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1930-2975&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1930-2975&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1930-2975&client=summon |