The time course of ineffective sham‐blinding during low‐intensity (1 mA) transcranial direct current stimulation
Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double‐blinded experimental des...
Saved in:
Published in | The European journal of neuroscience Vol. 50; no. 8; pp. 3380 - 3388 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
France
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
01.10.2019
John Wiley and Sons Inc |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0953-816X 1460-9568 1460-9568 |
DOI | 10.1111/ejn.14497 |
Cover
Abstract | Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double‐blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above‐chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham‐blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty‐two adults were tested in a preregistered, double‐blinded, within‐subjects design. A forced‐choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: “Is the stimulation on?” and “How sure are you?”. Distinct periods of non‐overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp‐down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham‐blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low‐intensity current strengths.
Low‐intensity (1 mA) anodal transcranial direct current stimulation was applied to the primary motor cortex for 10 min and compared to a 20 s sham protocol in the same individuals. Probe questions asked at regular intervals during the two protocols identified that participants were confident that the stimulation was active for a longer period during the 10 min stimulation compared to the 20 s sham protocol. We show here a failure of placebo control during the course of low‐intensity tDCS. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double-blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above-chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham-blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty-two adults were tested in a preregistered, double-blinded, within-subjects design. A forced-choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: "Is the stimulation on?" and "How sure are you?". Distinct periods of non-overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp-down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham-blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low-intensity current strengths. Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double‐blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above‐chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham‐blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty‐two adults were tested in a preregistered, double‐blinded, within‐subjects design. A forced‐choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: “Is the stimulation on?” and “How sure are you?”. Distinct periods of non‐overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp‐down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham‐blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low‐intensity current strengths. Low‐intensity (1 mA) anodal transcranial direct current stimulation was applied to the primary motor cortex for 10 min and compared to a 20 s sham protocol in the same individuals. Probe questions asked at regular intervals during the two protocols identified that participants were confident that the stimulation was active for a longer period during the 10 min stimulation compared to the 20 s sham protocol. We show here a failure of placebo control during the course of low‐intensity tDCS. Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double-blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above-chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham-blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty-two adults were tested in a preregistered, double-blinded, within-subjects design. A forced-choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: "Is the stimulation on?" and "How sure are you?". Distinct periods of non-overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp-down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham-blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low-intensity current strengths.Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double-blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above-chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham-blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty-two adults were tested in a preregistered, double-blinded, within-subjects design. A forced-choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: "Is the stimulation on?" and "How sure are you?". Distinct periods of non-overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp-down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham-blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low-intensity current strengths. Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double‐blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above‐chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham‐blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty‐two adults were tested in a preregistered, double‐blinded, within‐subjects design. A forced‐choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: “Is the stimulation on?” and “How sure are you?”. Distinct periods of non‐overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp‐down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham‐blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low‐intensity current strengths. |
Author | Buhôt, Larissa Greinacher, Robert Möller, Lisa Learmonth, Gemma |
AuthorAffiliation | 3 Department of Neurology University of Lübeck Lübeck Germany 1 School of Psychology University of Glasgow Glasgow UK 2 Quality and Usability Lab Technische Universität Berlin Berlin Germany 4 Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology University of Glasgow Glasgow UK |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: 4 Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology University of Glasgow Glasgow UK – name: 3 Department of Neurology University of Lübeck Lübeck Germany – name: 1 School of Psychology University of Glasgow Glasgow UK – name: 2 Quality and Usability Lab Technische Universität Berlin Berlin Germany |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Robert surname: Greinacher fullname: Greinacher, Robert organization: Technische Universität Berlin – sequence: 2 givenname: Larissa surname: Buhôt fullname: Buhôt, Larissa organization: University of Glasgow – sequence: 3 givenname: Lisa surname: Möller fullname: Möller, Lisa organization: University of Lübeck – sequence: 4 givenname: Gemma orcidid: 0000-0003-4061-4464 surname: Learmonth fullname: Learmonth, Gemma email: Gemma.Learmonth@glasgow.ac.uk organization: University of Glasgow |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31228880$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp1kU1uFDEQhS0URCaBBRdAltgki078027bG6QoCn-KYBMkdpbbXZ3xyO0Odnei2XEEjsBZOAonwclMEETghUuyv3p-rreHduIYAaHnlBzRso5hFY9oXWv5CC1o3ZBKi0btoAXRgleKNp930V7OK0KIamrxBO1yyphSiizQfLEEPPkBsBvnlAGPPfYR-h7c5K8B56Udfn791gYfOx8vcTen2xLGm3Lq4wQx-2mND-iP78PJIZ6SjdmVzduAO5-KCnZzShAnnMszc7CTH-NT9Li3IcOzbd1Hn16fXZy-rc4_vnl3enJeubrmstJcMmFdDbxj2golOidbKjqrpXTaCSCcsI72HcgeFGmbhiimCm-F5m3v-D56tdG9mtsBOldsJBvMVfKDTWszWm_-vol-aS7Ha9MorZWsi8DBViCNX2bIkxl8dhCCjTDO2TBWi4ZJykVBXz5AV2WisXzPME4JVbWUulAv_nT028p9IgU43gAujTkn6I3z093QikEfDCXmNnNTMjd3mZeOwwcd96L_YrfqNz7A-v-gOXv_YdPxC-R1wE4 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brs_2019_07_027 crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhad021 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2022_842954 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci12111545 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psc_2023_02_005 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci13030449 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15337 crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0374_22_2023 crossref_primary_10_3390_jpm14070675 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_16545_0 crossref_primary_10_1002_da_23004 crossref_primary_10_1002_ejp_1924 crossref_primary_10_5937_afmnai40_41738 crossref_primary_10_1111_ene_15163 crossref_primary_10_1080_09602011_2020_1767161 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15070 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2020_09_003 crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0126_20_2020 crossref_primary_10_1186_s42466_019_0042_0 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci12010058 crossref_primary_10_1111_jnp_12425 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pnpbp_2021_110261 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2021_07_013 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2024_1389651 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurom_2022_05_002 crossref_primary_10_1080_09602011_2020_1805335 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01490 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci12060738 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_apmr_2020_03_023 crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01591 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2019_146542 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15563 crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci11111526 crossref_primary_10_1186_s12888_023_05112_0 crossref_primary_10_1038_s41593_023_01456_8 crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2021_652393 crossref_primary_10_1007_s40519_020_00970_z crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brs_2020_07_006 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_16073 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_16603 crossref_primary_10_1051_bioconf_20237202001 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cnp_2022_05_002 |
Cites_doi | 10.3389/fneng.2014.00028 10.3389/fnins.2017.00664 10.1371/journal.pone.0163324 10.1111/ejn.14403 10.1371/journal.pone.0124509 10.1016/j.brs.2013.05.009 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.007 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.005 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 10.1016/j.brs.2011.12.001 10.1017/S1461145710001197 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000397 10.3389/fnins.2018.00410 10.1016/j.appet.2011.02.013 10.1007/s00221-016-4667-8 10.1111/ejn.12307 10.1016/j.brs.2013.01.005 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.01.004 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004 10.1155/2018/1237962 10.1162/089892903321662994 10.1371/journal.pone.0047514 10.1016/j.brs.2010.05.002 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00453 10.1177/1948550617697177 10.1016/j.brs.2008.04.003 10.1371/journal.pone.0149703 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003 10.1017/S1461145710001690 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2019 The Authors. published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd. 2019 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Copyright © 2019 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2019 The Authors. published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd. – notice: 2019 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd. – notice: Copyright © 2019 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd |
DBID | 24P AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QP 7QR 7TK 8FD FR3 P64 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1111/ejn.14497 |
DatabaseName | Wiley Online Library Open Access CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Chemoreception Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Technology Research Database Engineering Research Database Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Chemoreception Abstracts Engineering Research Database Technology Research Database Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef Chemoreception Abstracts |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: 24P name: Wiley Online Library Open Access url: https://authorservices.wiley.com/open-science/open-access/browse-journals.html sourceTypes: Publisher – sequence: 2 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Anatomy & Physiology Chemistry |
DocumentTitleAlternate | GREINACHER et al |
EISSN | 1460-9568 |
EndPage | 3388 |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC6899874 31228880 10_1111_ejn_14497 EJN14497 |
Genre | article Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Wellcome Trust grantid: 209209/Z/17/Z |
GroupedDBID | --- -~X .3N .GA .GJ .Y3 05W 0R~ 10A 1OB 1OC 24P 29G 31~ 33P 36B 3SF 4.4 50Y 50Z 51W 51X 52M 52N 52O 52P 52R 52S 52T 52U 52V 52W 52X 53G 5GY 5HH 5LA 5RE 5VS 66C 702 7PT 8-0 8-1 8-3 8-4 8-5 8UM 930 A01 A03 AAESR AAEVG AAHHS AAHQN AAIPD AAMNL AANHP AANLZ AAONW AASGY AAXRX AAYCA AAZKR ABCQN ABCUV ABDBF ABEML ABIVO ABJNI ABPVW ABQWH ABXGK ACAHQ ACBWZ ACCFJ ACCZN ACFBH ACGFS ACGOF ACIWK ACMXC ACPOU ACPRK ACRPL ACSCC ACUHS ACXBN ACXQS ACYXJ ADBBV ADBTR ADEOM ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADNMO ADOZA ADXAS ADZMN ADZOD AEEZP AEIGN AEIMD AENEX AEQDE AEUQT AEUYR AFBPY AFEBI AFFPM AFGKR AFPWT AFWVQ AFZJQ AHBTC AHEFC AIACR AITYG AIURR AIWBW AJBDE ALAGY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQN ALVPJ AMBMR AMYDB ASPBG ATUGU AVWKF AZBYB AZFZN AZVAB BAFTC BDRZF BFHJK BHBCM BMXJE BROTX BRXPI BY8 C45 CAG COF CS3 D-6 D-7 D-E D-F DC6 DCZOG DPXWK DR2 DRFUL DRMAN DRSTM EAD EAP EAS EBC EBD EBS EBX EJD EMB EMK EMOBN EPS ESX EX3 F00 F01 F04 F5P FEDTE FUBAC FZ0 G-S G.N GAKWD GODZA H.X HF~ HGLYW HVGLF HZI HZ~ IHE IX1 J0M K48 KBYEO LATKE LC2 LC3 LEEKS LH4 LITHE LOXES LP6 LP7 LUTES LW6 LYRES MEWTI MK4 MRFUL MRMAN MRSTM MSFUL MSMAN MSSTM MXFUL MXMAN MXSTM N04 N05 N9A NF~ O66 O9- OIG OVD P2P P2W P2X P2Z P4B P4D PALCI PQQKQ Q.N Q11 QB0 Q~Q R.K RIG RIWAO RJQFR ROL RX1 SAMSI SUPJJ SV3 TEORI TUS UB1 W8V W99 WBKPD WHG WIH WIJ WIK WNSPC WOHZO WOW WQJ WRC WUP WXI WXSBR WYISQ XG1 YFH ZGI ZZTAW ~IA ~WT AAYXX AEYWJ AGHNM AGQPQ AGYGG CITATION AAMMB AEFGJ AGXDD AIDQK AIDYY CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QP 7QR 7TK 8FD FR3 P64 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c4437-93725ac4e3d29a585dc7b15da977c9c5e0302d1fde7fe80b660828c4ea593bfc3 |
IEDL.DBID | DR2 |
ISSN | 0953-816X 1460-9568 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 14:14:18 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 11 11:23:22 EDT 2025 Sun Sep 07 23:56:42 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 06:02:55 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 03:02:20 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:11:50 EDT 2025 Wed Jan 22 16:38:15 EST 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | true |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 8 |
Keywords | reaction time sham tDCS placebo primary motor cortex |
Language | English |
License | Attribution 2019 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd. This is an open access article under the terms of the http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4437-93725ac4e3d29a585dc7b15da977c9c5e0302d1fde7fe80b660828c4ea593bfc3 |
Notes | https://publons.com/publon/10.1111/EJN.14497 The peer review history for this article is available at ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 The peer review history for this article is available at https://publons.com/publon/10.1111/EJN.14497 Edited by: Dr. Edmund Lalor. |
ORCID | 0000-0003-4061-4464 |
OpenAccessLink | https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2Fejn.14497 |
PMID | 31228880 |
PQID | 2310184779 |
PQPubID | 34057 |
PageCount | 9 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6899874 proquest_miscellaneous_2245627135 proquest_journals_2310184779 pubmed_primary_31228880 crossref_citationtrail_10_1111_ejn_14497 crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_14497 wiley_primary_10_1111_ejn_14497_EJN14497 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | October 2019 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2019-10-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 10 year: 2019 text: October 2019 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | France |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: France – name: Chichester – name: Hoboken |
PublicationTitle | The European journal of neuroscience |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Eur J Neurosci |
PublicationYear | 2019 |
Publisher | Wiley Subscription Services, Inc John Wiley and Sons Inc |
Publisher_xml | – name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc – name: John Wiley and Sons Inc |
References | 2017; 8 2009; 41 2015; 10 2016; 10 2003; 15 2011; 56 2007; 72 2011; 14 2011; 4 2008; 1 2006; 117 2013; 6 2016; 11 2015; 26 2015; 69 2018; 2018 2013; 38 2018; 119 2017; 11 2016; 234 2019 2009; 120 2018; 12 2012; 7 2014; 7 2012; 5 e_1_2_11_10_1 e_1_2_11_32_1 e_1_2_11_31_1 e_1_2_11_30_1 e_1_2_11_14_1 e_1_2_11_13_1 e_1_2_11_12_1 e_1_2_11_11_1 e_1_2_11_29_1 e_1_2_11_6_1 e_1_2_11_28_1 e_1_2_11_5_1 e_1_2_11_27_1 e_1_2_11_4_1 e_1_2_11_26_1 e_1_2_11_3_1 e_1_2_11_2_1 e_1_2_11_21_1 e_1_2_11_20_1 e_1_2_11_25_1 Conley A. C. (e_1_2_11_7_1) 2016; 10 e_1_2_11_24_1 e_1_2_11_9_1 e_1_2_11_23_1 e_1_2_11_8_1 e_1_2_11_22_1 e_1_2_11_18_1 e_1_2_11_17_1 e_1_2_11_16_1 e_1_2_11_15_1 e_1_2_11_19_1 |
References_xml | – volume: 234 start-page: 2629 year: 2016 end-page: 2642 article-title: Effects of a common transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) protocol on motor evoked potentials found to be highly variable within individuals over 9 testing sessions publication-title: Experimental Brain Research – volume: 14 start-page: 425 year: 2011 end-page: 426 article-title: Avoiding skin burns with transcranial direct current stimulation: Preliminary considerations publication-title: International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology – volume: 15 start-page: 619 year: 2003 end-page: 626 article-title: Facilitation of implicit motor learning by weak transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex in the human publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience – volume: 7 start-page: 1 year: 2014 end-page: 3 article-title: Reduced discomfort during high‐definition transcutaneous stimulation using 6% benzocaine publication-title: Frontiers in Neuroengineering – volume: 38 start-page: 2973 year: 2013 end-page: 2977 article-title: Challenges of proper placebo control for non‐invasive brain stimulation in clinical and experimental applications publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 1 start-page: 386 year: 2008 end-page: 387 article-title: Skin lesions after treatment with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) publication-title: Brain Stimulation – volume: 5 start-page: 499 year: 2012 end-page: 504 article-title: The fade‐in – Short stimulation – Fade out approach to sham tDCS – Reliable at 1 mA for naïve and experienced subjects, but not investigators publication-title: Brain Stimulation – volume: 119 start-page: 279554 year: 2018 article-title: Expectations may influence the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation publication-title: Neuropsychologia – volume: 14 start-page: 1133 year: 2011 end-page: 1145 article-title: A systematic review on reporting and assessment of adverse effects associated with transcranial direct current stimulation publication-title: International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology – volume: 120 start-page: 2008 year: 2009 end-page: 2039 article-title: Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology – volume: 4 start-page: 38 year: 2011 end-page: 42 article-title: Reducing procedural pain and discomfort associated with transcranial direct current stimulation James publication-title: Brain Stimulation – volume: 7 start-page: e47514 year: 2012 article-title: Rethinking clinical trials of transcranial direct current stimulation: Participant and assessor blinding is inadequate at intensities of 2 mA publication-title: PLoS ONE – volume: 11 start-page: 1 year: 2017 end-page: 13 article-title: No interaction between tDCS current strength and baseline performance: A conceptual replication publication-title: Frontiers in Neuroscience – volume: 6 start-page: 690 year: 2013 end-page: 695 article-title: Evaluation of sham transcranial direct current stimulation for randomized, placebo‐controlled clinical trials publication-title: Brain Stimulation – volume: 117 start-page: 845 year: 2006 end-page: 850 article-title: Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): A tool for double‐blind sham‐controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology – volume: 12 start-page: 1 year: 2018 end-page: 17 article-title: Differential bilateral primary motor cortex tDCS fails to modulate choice bias and readiness in perceptual decision making publication-title: Frontiers in Neuroscience – volume: 11 start-page: 1 year: 2016 end-page: 16 article-title: Perceived comfort and blinding efficacy in randomised sham‐controlled transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) trials at 2 mA in young and older healthy adults publication-title: PLoS ONE – volume: 10 start-page: 1 year: 2016 end-page: 13 article-title: No effect of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the motor cortex on response‐related ERPs during a conflict task publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience – volume: 56 start-page: 741 year: 2011 end-page: 746 article-title: Prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) temporarily reduces food cravings and increases the self‐reported ability to resist food in adults with frequent food craving publication-title: Appetite – volume: 2018 start-page: 1 year: 2018 end-page: 14 article-title: Anodal tDCS over primary motor cortex provides no advantage to learning motor sequences via observation publication-title: Neural Plasticity – volume: 1 start-page: 206 year: 2008 end-page: 223 article-title: Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008 publication-title: Brain Stimulation – volume: 10 start-page: 1 year: 2015 end-page: 14 article-title: Anodal tDCS over the motor cortex on prepared and unprepared responses in young adults publication-title: PLoS ONE – volume: 69 start-page: 152 year: 2015 end-page: 165 article-title: Non‐linear effects of transcranial direct current stimulation as a function of individual baseline performance: Evidence from biparietal tDCS influence on lateralized attention bias publication-title: Cortex – year: 2019 article-title: Blinding is compromised for transcranial direct current stimulation at 1 mA for 20 minutes in young healthy adults publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience – volume: 26 start-page: 618 year: 2015 end-page: 622 article-title: Transcranial direct current stimulation: Before, during, or after motor training? publication-title: NeuroReport – volume: 8 start-page: 355 year: 2017 end-page: 362 article-title: Equivalence tests: A practical primer for tests, correlations, and meta‐analyses publication-title: Social Psychological and Personality Science – volume: 10 start-page: 453 year: 2016 article-title: The importance of sample size for reproducibility of tDCS effects publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience – volume: 6 start-page: 946 year: 2013 end-page: 951 article-title: Perception of comfort during active and sham transcranial direct current stimulation: A double blind study publication-title: Brain Stimulation – volume: 11 start-page: 1 year: 2016 end-page: 13 article-title: Are participants aware of the type and intensity of transcranial direct current stimulation? publication-title: PLoS ONE – volume: 41 start-page: 1149 year: 2009 end-page: 1160 article-title: Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses publication-title: Behavior Research Methods – volume: 72 start-page: 208 year: 2007 end-page: 214 article-title: Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients publication-title: Brain Research Bulletin – ident: e_1_2_11_13_1 doi: 10.3389/fneng.2014.00028 – ident: e_1_2_11_16_1 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00664 – ident: e_1_2_11_29_1 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163324 – ident: e_1_2_11_30_1 doi: 10.1111/ejn.14403 – ident: e_1_2_11_8_1 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124509 – ident: e_1_2_11_28_1 doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.05.009 – ident: e_1_2_11_4_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.007 – ident: e_1_2_11_26_1 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.005 – ident: e_1_2_11_10_1 doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 – volume: 10 start-page: 1 year: 2016 ident: e_1_2_11_7_1 article-title: No effect of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the motor cortex on response‐related ERPs during a conflict task publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience – ident: e_1_2_11_2_1 doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.12.001 – ident: e_1_2_11_17_1 doi: 10.1017/S1461145710001197 – ident: e_1_2_11_6_1 doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000397 – ident: e_1_2_11_31_1 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00410 – ident: e_1_2_11_12_1 doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.02.013 – ident: e_1_2_11_14_1 doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4667-8 – ident: e_1_2_11_9_1 doi: 10.1111/ejn.12307 – ident: e_1_2_11_24_1 doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.01.005 – ident: e_1_2_11_25_1 doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.01.004 – ident: e_1_2_11_20_1 doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004 – ident: e_1_2_11_3_1 doi: 10.1155/2018/1237962 – ident: e_1_2_11_21_1 doi: 10.1162/089892903321662994 – ident: e_1_2_11_22_1 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047514 – ident: e_1_2_11_18_1 doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2010.05.002 – ident: e_1_2_11_27_1 doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016 – ident: e_1_2_11_19_1 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00453 – ident: e_1_2_11_15_1 doi: 10.1177/1948550617697177 – ident: e_1_2_11_23_1 doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.04.003 – ident: e_1_2_11_32_1 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149703 – ident: e_1_2_11_11_1 doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003 – ident: e_1_2_11_5_1 doi: 10.1017/S1461145710001690 |
SSID | ssj0008645 |
Score | 2.502215 |
Snippet | Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed crossref wiley |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 3380 |
SubjectTerms | Adult Awareness Cognitive Neuroscience Cortex (motor) Double-Blind Method Electrical stimulation of the brain Electrical stimuli ESB Female Humans Male Motor Cortex Perception placebo primary motor cortex Protocol reaction time Reaction time task Research Report Scalp sham tDCS Time Factors Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation - adverse effects Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation - methods Young Adult |
Title | The time course of ineffective sham‐blinding during low‐intensity (1 mA) transcranial direct current stimulation |
URI | https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2Fejn.14497 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31228880 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2310184779 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2245627135 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6899874 |
Volume | 50 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3datRAFD7U3uiNP60_0VpGEakXKclkkuzg1VJbSsEiYmEvhDAzmdDV3aR0s0i98hF8BJ_FR_FJPGcmiV2rIN4sy-aEJLPn5zuZc74D8MxIovHKVcgVpqvCZFEoy4y4CG0UaYMxnlPv8Ovj7PBEHE3SyRq87HthPD_E8MKNLMP5azJwpReXjNx-qGlnUlIneZxkxJv_6u0v6qhR5gYUE51aOIqzSccqRFU8w5mrsegKwLxaJ3kZv7oAdHAL3ve37utOPu4uW71rPv_G6vifz3YbbnbAlI29Jt2BNVtvwOa4xqR8fsGeM1cq6t7Bb8D1vX5M3CYsUdEYTahnpqGCENZUDK_vykTQk7LFqZr_-PJVz6auf4b5vkg2az7hr1NfQN9esJ34-7f5-AVrKXYa_EC7YD7eMuM5pBh6o3k3bewunBzsv9s7DLtZDqERIslDREE8VUbYpORSYY5SmlzHaakQfxppUovOhpdxVdq8sqNIZxlx66G8SmWiK5Pcg_W6qe0DYNIoR8qjK64EZVxCazxHiMgjpgB2-n-1MB3ROc3bmBV9woPLW7jlDeDpIHrm2T3-JLTVq0bRGfiiIFiMyXGeywCeDIdx6Wm_RdW2WaIMbSpzmoEYwH2vScNVkpjzEfrOAPIVHRsEiPZ79Ug9PXX03xmlyLnAx3Qq9PcbL_aPjt2Xh_8u-ghuICSUvlxxC9bb86V9jLCr1dtwjYs3287KfgKx8C6h |
linkProvider | Wiley-Blackwell |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3NbtQwEB6VciiXAi0_gQIGIVQOqfLjJGuJy6pqtZR2D6iV9oIi23HUhd2kolmhcuIReASehUfhSZixk9ClICEuq9VmonWc-fnGHn8D8FwLovHKpB9JTFe5TgNfFClxEZogUBpjfERnh4_G6eiEH0ySyQq86s7COH6IfsGNLMP6azJwWpC-ZOXmfUVbkyK7Btft_hxBore_yKMGqW1RTIRq_iBMJy2vENXx9LcuR6MrEPNqpeRlBGtD0P5NeNcN3lWefNhZNGpHf_6N1_F_n-4WrLfYlA2dMt2GFVNtwOawwrx8fsFeMFstapfhN2Btt-sUtwkL1DVGTeqZrqkmhNUlwwHYShF0puz8VM5_fPmqZlN7hIa5o5FsVn_CX6euhr65YNvh92_z4UvWUPjU-IGmwVzIZdrRSDF0SPO24dgdONnfO94d-W07B19zHmc-AqEokZqbuIiExDSl0JkKk0IiBNVCJwb9TVSEZWGy0gwClaZEr4fyMhGxKnV8F1arujL3gQktLS-PKiPJKeniSuE9nAcONHmw3b3WXLdc59RyY5Z3OQ9Ob26n14NnveiZI_j4k9BWpxt5a-PnOSFjzI-zTHjwtL-MU09bLrIy9QJlaF85ojaIHtxzqtT_SxxG0QDdpwfZkpL1AsT8vXylmp5aBvCUsuSM42NaHfr7wPO9g7H98uDfRZ_A2uj46DA_fD1-8xBuIEIUrnpxC1abjwvzCFFYox5bY_sJTvwx6A |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV3NbtNAEB6VIkEvCFoohgILQqgcLNnrtTcrTlFpVApEPVApN2u9XqtBiV21iVBvPEIfoc_Co_RJOrNrW40KEpcoiseK7fn7xjv7DcB7o4jGS-qQayxXhcmiUJUZcRHaKCoM5nhOe4e_j7ODY3E4SSdr8KnbC-P5IfoXbuQZLl6Tg5-W1S0ntz9rWplU8h7cF2h3ZN5cHPVheJC5CcXEpxYO4mzS0gpRG09_6moyuoMw7zZK3gawLgONHsOjFjqyodf1E1iz9SZsDWssm-cX7ANzzZzuLfkmPNzrBrltwRJNgdEMeWYaatlgTcXw7l0jB8Y6dn6i59e_L4vZ1O1wYX7nIps1v_DXqW9xX1yw3fjP1Xz4kS0ouxn8QMtlPiMy41meGMaLeTsP7Ckcj_Z_7B2E7bSF0AiRyBBxCk-1ETYpudJYRZRGFnFaakSIRpnUYjjgZVyVVlZ2EBVZRux3KK9TlRSVSZ7Bet3U9jkwZbSjzSkqrgXVRKIo8BwhIo9pAtjtHntuWipymogxy7uSBDWUOw0F8K4XPfX8G38T2ul0l7cueJ4TcMXyVUoVwNv-MD56WhHRtW2WKEPLvpymFAaw7VXd_0sScz7A6BaAXDGCXoCIuVeP1NMTR9CdURErBd6mM5d_X3i-fzh2X178v-gbeHD0eZR_-zL--hI2EL8p31u4A-uLs6V9hRhpUbx2vnADADcQXw |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+time+course+of+ineffective+sham-blinding+during+low-intensity+%281+mA%29+transcranial+direct+current+stimulation&rft.jtitle=The+European+journal+of+neuroscience&rft.au=Greinacher%2C+Robert&rft.au=Buh%C3%B4t%2C+Larissa&rft.au=M%C3%B6ller%2C+Lisa&rft.au=Learmonth%2C+Gemma&rft.date=2019-10-01&rft.issn=1460-9568&rft.eissn=1460-9568&rft.volume=50&rft.issue=8&rft.spage=3380&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2Fejn.14497&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0953-816X&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0953-816X&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0953-816X&client=summon |