The time course of ineffective sham‐blinding during low‐intensity (1 mA) transcranial direct current stimulation

Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double‐blinded experimental des...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inThe European journal of neuroscience Vol. 50; no. 8; pp. 3380 - 3388
Main Authors Greinacher, Robert, Buhôt, Larissa, Möller, Lisa, Learmonth, Gemma
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published France Wiley Subscription Services, Inc 01.10.2019
John Wiley and Sons Inc
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN0953-816X
1460-9568
1460-9568
DOI10.1111/ejn.14497

Cover

Abstract Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double‐blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above‐chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham‐blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty‐two adults were tested in a preregistered, double‐blinded, within‐subjects design. A forced‐choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: “Is the stimulation on?” and “How sure are you?”. Distinct periods of non‐overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp‐down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham‐blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low‐intensity current strengths. Low‐intensity (1 mA) anodal transcranial direct current stimulation was applied to the primary motor cortex for 10 min and compared to a 20 s sham protocol in the same individuals. Probe questions asked at regular intervals during the two protocols identified that participants were confident that the stimulation was active for a longer period during the 10 min stimulation compared to the 20 s sham protocol. We show here a failure of placebo control during the course of low‐intensity tDCS.
AbstractList Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double-blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above-chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham-blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty-two adults were tested in a preregistered, double-blinded, within-subjects design. A forced-choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: "Is the stimulation on?" and "How sure are you?". Distinct periods of non-overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp-down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham-blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low-intensity current strengths.
Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double‐blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above‐chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham‐blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty‐two adults were tested in a preregistered, double‐blinded, within‐subjects design. A forced‐choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: “Is the stimulation on?” and “How sure are you?”. Distinct periods of non‐overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp‐down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham‐blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low‐intensity current strengths. Low‐intensity (1 mA) anodal transcranial direct current stimulation was applied to the primary motor cortex for 10 min and compared to a 20 s sham protocol in the same individuals. Probe questions asked at regular intervals during the two protocols identified that participants were confident that the stimulation was active for a longer period during the 10 min stimulation compared to the 20 s sham protocol. We show here a failure of placebo control during the course of low‐intensity tDCS.
Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double-blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above-chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham-blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty-two adults were tested in a preregistered, double-blinded, within-subjects design. A forced-choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: "Is the stimulation on?" and "How sure are you?". Distinct periods of non-overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp-down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham-blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low-intensity current strengths.Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double-blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above-chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham-blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty-two adults were tested in a preregistered, double-blinded, within-subjects design. A forced-choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: "Is the stimulation on?" and "How sure are you?". Distinct periods of non-overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp-down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham-blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low-intensity current strengths.
Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols are presumed to be perceptually identical on the scalp, and thus represent an effective method of delivering double‐blinded experimental designs. However, participants often show above‐chance accuracy when asked which condition involved active/sham retrospectively. We assessed the time course of sham‐blinding during active and sham tDCS. We predicted that participants would be aware that the current is switched on for longer in the active versus sham protocol. Thirty‐two adults were tested in a preregistered, double‐blinded, within‐subjects design. A forced‐choice reaction time task was undertaken before, during and after active (10 min 1 mA) and sham (20 s 1 mA) tDCS. The anode was placed over the left primary motor cortex (C3) to target the right hand, and the cathode on the right forehead. Two probe questions were asked every 30 s: “Is the stimulation on?” and “How sure are you?”. Distinct periods of non‐overlapping confidence intervals were identified between conditions, totalling 5 min (57.1% of the total difference in stimulation time). These began immediately after sham ramp‐down and lasted until the active protocol had ended. We therefore show a failure of placebo control during 1 mA tDCS. These results highlight the need to develop more effective methods of sham‐blinding during transcranial electrical stimulation protocols, even when delivered at low‐intensity current strengths.
Author Buhôt, Larissa
Greinacher, Robert
Möller, Lisa
Learmonth, Gemma
AuthorAffiliation 3 Department of Neurology University of Lübeck Lübeck Germany
1 School of Psychology University of Glasgow Glasgow UK
2 Quality and Usability Lab Technische Universität Berlin Berlin Germany
4 Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology University of Glasgow Glasgow UK
AuthorAffiliation_xml – name: 4 Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology University of Glasgow Glasgow UK
– name: 3 Department of Neurology University of Lübeck Lübeck Germany
– name: 1 School of Psychology University of Glasgow Glasgow UK
– name: 2 Quality and Usability Lab Technische Universität Berlin Berlin Germany
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Robert
  surname: Greinacher
  fullname: Greinacher, Robert
  organization: Technische Universität Berlin
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Larissa
  surname: Buhôt
  fullname: Buhôt, Larissa
  organization: University of Glasgow
– sequence: 3
  givenname: Lisa
  surname: Möller
  fullname: Möller, Lisa
  organization: University of Lübeck
– sequence: 4
  givenname: Gemma
  orcidid: 0000-0003-4061-4464
  surname: Learmonth
  fullname: Learmonth, Gemma
  email: Gemma.Learmonth@glasgow.ac.uk
  organization: University of Glasgow
BackLink https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31228880$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp1kU1uFDEQhS0URCaBBRdAltgki078027bG6QoCn-KYBMkdpbbXZ3xyO0Odnei2XEEjsBZOAonwclMEETghUuyv3p-rreHduIYAaHnlBzRso5hFY9oXWv5CC1o3ZBKi0btoAXRgleKNp930V7OK0KIamrxBO1yyphSiizQfLEEPPkBsBvnlAGPPfYR-h7c5K8B56Udfn791gYfOx8vcTen2xLGm3Lq4wQx-2mND-iP78PJIZ6SjdmVzduAO5-KCnZzShAnnMszc7CTH-NT9Li3IcOzbd1Hn16fXZy-rc4_vnl3enJeubrmstJcMmFdDbxj2golOidbKjqrpXTaCSCcsI72HcgeFGmbhiimCm-F5m3v-D56tdG9mtsBOldsJBvMVfKDTWszWm_-vol-aS7Ha9MorZWsi8DBViCNX2bIkxl8dhCCjTDO2TBWi4ZJykVBXz5AV2WisXzPME4JVbWUulAv_nT028p9IgU43gAujTkn6I3z093QikEfDCXmNnNTMjd3mZeOwwcd96L_YrfqNz7A-v-gOXv_YdPxC-R1wE4
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brs_2019_07_027
crossref_primary_10_1093_cercor_bhad021
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2022_842954
crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci12111545
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_psc_2023_02_005
crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci13030449
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15337
crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0374_22_2023
crossref_primary_10_3390_jpm14070675
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41598_022_16545_0
crossref_primary_10_1002_da_23004
crossref_primary_10_1002_ejp_1924
crossref_primary_10_5937_afmnai40_41738
crossref_primary_10_1111_ene_15163
crossref_primary_10_1080_09602011_2020_1767161
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15070
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neuroscience_2020_09_003
crossref_primary_10_1523_ENEURO_0126_20_2020
crossref_primary_10_1186_s42466_019_0042_0
crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci12010058
crossref_primary_10_1111_jnp_12425
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_pnpbp_2021_110261
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neubiorev_2021_07_013
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2024_1389651
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_neurom_2022_05_002
crossref_primary_10_1080_09602011_2020_1805335
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01490
crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci12060738
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_apmr_2020_03_023
crossref_primary_10_1162_jocn_a_01591
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brainres_2019_146542
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_15563
crossref_primary_10_3390_brainsci11111526
crossref_primary_10_1186_s12888_023_05112_0
crossref_primary_10_1038_s41593_023_01456_8
crossref_primary_10_3389_fnhum_2021_652393
crossref_primary_10_1007_s40519_020_00970_z
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_brs_2020_07_006
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_16073
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_16603
crossref_primary_10_1051_bioconf_20237202001
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cnp_2022_05_002
Cites_doi 10.3389/fneng.2014.00028
10.3389/fnins.2017.00664
10.1371/journal.pone.0163324
10.1111/ejn.14403
10.1371/journal.pone.0124509
10.1016/j.brs.2013.05.009
10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.007
10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.005
10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
10.1016/j.brs.2011.12.001
10.1017/S1461145710001197
10.1097/WNR.0000000000000397
10.3389/fnins.2018.00410
10.1016/j.appet.2011.02.013
10.1007/s00221-016-4667-8
10.1111/ejn.12307
10.1016/j.brs.2013.01.005
10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.01.004
10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004
10.1155/2018/1237962
10.1162/089892903321662994
10.1371/journal.pone.0047514
10.1016/j.brs.2010.05.002
10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016
10.3389/fnhum.2016.00453
10.1177/1948550617697177
10.1016/j.brs.2008.04.003
10.1371/journal.pone.0149703
10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003
10.1017/S1461145710001690
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2019 The Authors. published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
2019 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Copyright © 2019 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Copyright_xml – notice: 2019 The Authors. published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
– notice: 2019 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
– notice: Copyright © 2019 Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
DBID 24P
AAYXX
CITATION
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QP
7QR
7TK
8FD
FR3
P64
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1111/ejn.14497
DatabaseName Wiley Online Library Open Access
CrossRef
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Chemoreception Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Technology Research Database
Engineering Research Database
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Chemoreception Abstracts
Engineering Research Database
Technology Research Database
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
Biotechnology and BioEngineering Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE

MEDLINE - Academic
CrossRef
Chemoreception Abstracts

Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: 24P
  name: Wiley Online Library Open Access
  url: https://authorservices.wiley.com/open-science/open-access/browse-journals.html
  sourceTypes: Publisher
– sequence: 2
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Anatomy & Physiology
Chemistry
DocumentTitleAlternate GREINACHER et al
EISSN 1460-9568
EndPage 3388
ExternalDocumentID PMC6899874
31228880
10_1111_ejn_14497
EJN14497
Genre article
Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Wellcome Trust
  grantid: 209209/Z/17/Z
GroupedDBID ---
-~X
.3N
.GA
.GJ
.Y3
05W
0R~
10A
1OB
1OC
24P
29G
31~
33P
36B
3SF
4.4
50Y
50Z
51W
51X
52M
52N
52O
52P
52R
52S
52T
52U
52V
52W
52X
53G
5GY
5HH
5LA
5RE
5VS
66C
702
7PT
8-0
8-1
8-3
8-4
8-5
8UM
930
A01
A03
AAESR
AAEVG
AAHHS
AAHQN
AAIPD
AAMNL
AANHP
AANLZ
AAONW
AASGY
AAXRX
AAYCA
AAZKR
ABCQN
ABCUV
ABDBF
ABEML
ABIVO
ABJNI
ABPVW
ABQWH
ABXGK
ACAHQ
ACBWZ
ACCFJ
ACCZN
ACFBH
ACGFS
ACGOF
ACIWK
ACMXC
ACPOU
ACPRK
ACRPL
ACSCC
ACUHS
ACXBN
ACXQS
ACYXJ
ADBBV
ADBTR
ADEOM
ADIZJ
ADKYN
ADMGS
ADNMO
ADOZA
ADXAS
ADZMN
ADZOD
AEEZP
AEIGN
AEIMD
AENEX
AEQDE
AEUQT
AEUYR
AFBPY
AFEBI
AFFPM
AFGKR
AFPWT
AFWVQ
AFZJQ
AHBTC
AHEFC
AIACR
AITYG
AIURR
AIWBW
AJBDE
ALAGY
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALUQN
ALVPJ
AMBMR
AMYDB
ASPBG
ATUGU
AVWKF
AZBYB
AZFZN
AZVAB
BAFTC
BDRZF
BFHJK
BHBCM
BMXJE
BROTX
BRXPI
BY8
C45
CAG
COF
CS3
D-6
D-7
D-E
D-F
DC6
DCZOG
DPXWK
DR2
DRFUL
DRMAN
DRSTM
EAD
EAP
EAS
EBC
EBD
EBS
EBX
EJD
EMB
EMK
EMOBN
EPS
ESX
EX3
F00
F01
F04
F5P
FEDTE
FUBAC
FZ0
G-S
G.N
GAKWD
GODZA
H.X
HF~
HGLYW
HVGLF
HZI
HZ~
IHE
IX1
J0M
K48
KBYEO
LATKE
LC2
LC3
LEEKS
LH4
LITHE
LOXES
LP6
LP7
LUTES
LW6
LYRES
MEWTI
MK4
MRFUL
MRMAN
MRSTM
MSFUL
MSMAN
MSSTM
MXFUL
MXMAN
MXSTM
N04
N05
N9A
NF~
O66
O9-
OIG
OVD
P2P
P2W
P2X
P2Z
P4B
P4D
PALCI
PQQKQ
Q.N
Q11
QB0
Q~Q
R.K
RIG
RIWAO
RJQFR
ROL
RX1
SAMSI
SUPJJ
SV3
TEORI
TUS
UB1
W8V
W99
WBKPD
WHG
WIH
WIJ
WIK
WNSPC
WOHZO
WOW
WQJ
WRC
WUP
WXI
WXSBR
WYISQ
XG1
YFH
ZGI
ZZTAW
~IA
~WT
AAYXX
AEYWJ
AGHNM
AGQPQ
AGYGG
CITATION
AAMMB
AEFGJ
AGXDD
AIDQK
AIDYY
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QP
7QR
7TK
8FD
FR3
P64
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c4437-93725ac4e3d29a585dc7b15da977c9c5e0302d1fde7fe80b660828c4ea593bfc3
IEDL.DBID DR2
ISSN 0953-816X
1460-9568
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 14:14:18 EDT 2025
Fri Jul 11 11:23:22 EDT 2025
Sun Sep 07 23:56:42 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 06:02:55 EDT 2025
Tue Jul 01 03:02:20 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:11:50 EDT 2025
Wed Jan 22 16:38:15 EST 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess true
IsOpenAccess true
IsPeerReviewed true
IsScholarly true
Issue 8
Keywords reaction time
sham
tDCS
placebo
primary motor cortex
Language English
License Attribution
2019 The Authors. European Journal of Neuroscience published by Federation of European Neuroscience Societies and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
This is an open access article under the terms of the http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4437-93725ac4e3d29a585dc7b15da977c9c5e0302d1fde7fe80b660828c4ea593bfc3
Notes https://publons.com/publon/10.1111/EJN.14497
The peer review history for this article is available at
ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
The peer review history for this article is available at https://publons.com/publon/10.1111/EJN.14497
Edited by: Dr. Edmund Lalor.
ORCID 0000-0003-4061-4464
OpenAccessLink https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2Fejn.14497
PMID 31228880
PQID 2310184779
PQPubID 34057
PageCount 9
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_6899874
proquest_miscellaneous_2245627135
proquest_journals_2310184779
pubmed_primary_31228880
crossref_citationtrail_10_1111_ejn_14497
crossref_primary_10_1111_ejn_14497
wiley_primary_10_1111_ejn_14497_EJN14497
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate October 2019
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2019-10-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 10
  year: 2019
  text: October 2019
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace France
PublicationPlace_xml – name: France
– name: Chichester
– name: Hoboken
PublicationTitle The European journal of neuroscience
PublicationTitleAlternate Eur J Neurosci
PublicationYear 2019
Publisher Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
John Wiley and Sons Inc
Publisher_xml – name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
– name: John Wiley and Sons Inc
References 2017; 8
2009; 41
2015; 10
2016; 10
2003; 15
2011; 56
2007; 72
2011; 14
2011; 4
2008; 1
2006; 117
2013; 6
2016; 11
2015; 26
2015; 69
2018; 2018
2013; 38
2018; 119
2017; 11
2016; 234
2019
2009; 120
2018; 12
2012; 7
2014; 7
2012; 5
e_1_2_11_10_1
e_1_2_11_32_1
e_1_2_11_31_1
e_1_2_11_30_1
e_1_2_11_14_1
e_1_2_11_13_1
e_1_2_11_12_1
e_1_2_11_11_1
e_1_2_11_29_1
e_1_2_11_6_1
e_1_2_11_28_1
e_1_2_11_5_1
e_1_2_11_27_1
e_1_2_11_4_1
e_1_2_11_26_1
e_1_2_11_3_1
e_1_2_11_2_1
e_1_2_11_21_1
e_1_2_11_20_1
e_1_2_11_25_1
Conley A. C. (e_1_2_11_7_1) 2016; 10
e_1_2_11_24_1
e_1_2_11_9_1
e_1_2_11_23_1
e_1_2_11_8_1
e_1_2_11_22_1
e_1_2_11_18_1
e_1_2_11_17_1
e_1_2_11_16_1
e_1_2_11_15_1
e_1_2_11_19_1
References_xml – volume: 234
  start-page: 2629
  year: 2016
  end-page: 2642
  article-title: Effects of a common transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) protocol on motor evoked potentials found to be highly variable within individuals over 9 testing sessions
  publication-title: Experimental Brain Research
– volume: 14
  start-page: 425
  year: 2011
  end-page: 426
  article-title: Avoiding skin burns with transcranial direct current stimulation: Preliminary considerations
  publication-title: International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology
– volume: 15
  start-page: 619
  year: 2003
  end-page: 626
  article-title: Facilitation of implicit motor learning by weak transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex in the human
  publication-title: Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience
– volume: 7
  start-page: 1
  year: 2014
  end-page: 3
  article-title: Reduced discomfort during high‐definition transcutaneous stimulation using 6% benzocaine
  publication-title: Frontiers in Neuroengineering
– volume: 38
  start-page: 2973
  year: 2013
  end-page: 2977
  article-title: Challenges of proper placebo control for non‐invasive brain stimulation in clinical and experimental applications
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 1
  start-page: 386
  year: 2008
  end-page: 387
  article-title: Skin lesions after treatment with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)
  publication-title: Brain Stimulation
– volume: 5
  start-page: 499
  year: 2012
  end-page: 504
  article-title: The fade‐in – Short stimulation – Fade out approach to sham tDCS – Reliable at 1 mA for naïve and experienced subjects, but not investigators
  publication-title: Brain Stimulation
– volume: 119
  start-page: 279554
  year: 2018
  article-title: Expectations may influence the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation
  publication-title: Neuropsychologia
– volume: 14
  start-page: 1133
  year: 2011
  end-page: 1145
  article-title: A systematic review on reporting and assessment of adverse effects associated with transcranial direct current stimulation
  publication-title: International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology
– volume: 120
  start-page: 2008
  year: 2009
  end-page: 2039
  article-title: Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research
  publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology
– volume: 4
  start-page: 38
  year: 2011
  end-page: 42
  article-title: Reducing procedural pain and discomfort associated with transcranial direct current stimulation James
  publication-title: Brain Stimulation
– volume: 7
  start-page: e47514
  year: 2012
  article-title: Rethinking clinical trials of transcranial direct current stimulation: Participant and assessor blinding is inadequate at intensities of 2 mA
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 11
  start-page: 1
  year: 2017
  end-page: 13
  article-title: No interaction between tDCS current strength and baseline performance: A conceptual replication
  publication-title: Frontiers in Neuroscience
– volume: 6
  start-page: 690
  year: 2013
  end-page: 695
  article-title: Evaluation of sham transcranial direct current stimulation for randomized, placebo‐controlled clinical trials
  publication-title: Brain Stimulation
– volume: 117
  start-page: 845
  year: 2006
  end-page: 850
  article-title: Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): A tool for double‐blind sham‐controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation
  publication-title: Clinical Neurophysiology
– volume: 12
  start-page: 1
  year: 2018
  end-page: 17
  article-title: Differential bilateral primary motor cortex tDCS fails to modulate choice bias and readiness in perceptual decision making
  publication-title: Frontiers in Neuroscience
– volume: 11
  start-page: 1
  year: 2016
  end-page: 16
  article-title: Perceived comfort and blinding efficacy in randomised sham‐controlled transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) trials at 2 mA in young and older healthy adults
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 10
  start-page: 1
  year: 2016
  end-page: 13
  article-title: No effect of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the motor cortex on response‐related ERPs during a conflict task
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
– volume: 56
  start-page: 741
  year: 2011
  end-page: 746
  article-title: Prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) temporarily reduces food cravings and increases the self‐reported ability to resist food in adults with frequent food craving
  publication-title: Appetite
– volume: 2018
  start-page: 1
  year: 2018
  end-page: 14
  article-title: Anodal tDCS over primary motor cortex provides no advantage to learning motor sequences via observation
  publication-title: Neural Plasticity
– volume: 1
  start-page: 206
  year: 2008
  end-page: 223
  article-title: Transcranial direct current stimulation: State of the art 2008
  publication-title: Brain Stimulation
– volume: 10
  start-page: 1
  year: 2015
  end-page: 14
  article-title: Anodal tDCS over the motor cortex on prepared and unprepared responses in young adults
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 69
  start-page: 152
  year: 2015
  end-page: 165
  article-title: Non‐linear effects of transcranial direct current stimulation as a function of individual baseline performance: Evidence from biparietal tDCS influence on lateralized attention bias
  publication-title: Cortex
– year: 2019
  article-title: Blinding is compromised for transcranial direct current stimulation at 1 mA for 20 minutes in young healthy adults
  publication-title: European Journal of Neuroscience
– volume: 26
  start-page: 618
  year: 2015
  end-page: 622
  article-title: Transcranial direct current stimulation: Before, during, or after motor training?
  publication-title: NeuroReport
– volume: 8
  start-page: 355
  year: 2017
  end-page: 362
  article-title: Equivalence tests: A practical primer for tests, correlations, and meta‐analyses
  publication-title: Social Psychological and Personality Science
– volume: 10
  start-page: 453
  year: 2016
  article-title: The importance of sample size for reproducibility of tDCS effects
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
– volume: 6
  start-page: 946
  year: 2013
  end-page: 951
  article-title: Perception of comfort during active and sham transcranial direct current stimulation: A double blind study
  publication-title: Brain Stimulation
– volume: 11
  start-page: 1
  year: 2016
  end-page: 13
  article-title: Are participants aware of the type and intensity of transcranial direct current stimulation?
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 41
  start-page: 1149
  year: 2009
  end-page: 1160
  article-title: Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses
  publication-title: Behavior Research Methods
– volume: 72
  start-page: 208
  year: 2007
  end-page: 214
  article-title: Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients
  publication-title: Brain Research Bulletin
– ident: e_1_2_11_13_1
  doi: 10.3389/fneng.2014.00028
– ident: e_1_2_11_16_1
  doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00664
– ident: e_1_2_11_29_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0163324
– ident: e_1_2_11_30_1
  doi: 10.1111/ejn.14403
– ident: e_1_2_11_8_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0124509
– ident: e_1_2_11_28_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.05.009
– ident: e_1_2_11_4_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.007
– ident: e_1_2_11_26_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.09.005
– ident: e_1_2_11_10_1
  doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
– volume: 10
  start-page: 1
  year: 2016
  ident: e_1_2_11_7_1
  article-title: No effect of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the motor cortex on response‐related ERPs during a conflict task
  publication-title: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
– ident: e_1_2_11_2_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.12.001
– ident: e_1_2_11_17_1
  doi: 10.1017/S1461145710001197
– ident: e_1_2_11_6_1
  doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000397
– ident: e_1_2_11_31_1
  doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00410
– ident: e_1_2_11_12_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.02.013
– ident: e_1_2_11_14_1
  doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4667-8
– ident: e_1_2_11_9_1
  doi: 10.1111/ejn.12307
– ident: e_1_2_11_24_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.01.005
– ident: e_1_2_11_25_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.01.004
– ident: e_1_2_11_20_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004
– ident: e_1_2_11_3_1
  doi: 10.1155/2018/1237962
– ident: e_1_2_11_21_1
  doi: 10.1162/089892903321662994
– ident: e_1_2_11_22_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047514
– ident: e_1_2_11_18_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2010.05.002
– ident: e_1_2_11_27_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016
– ident: e_1_2_11_19_1
  doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00453
– ident: e_1_2_11_15_1
  doi: 10.1177/1948550617697177
– ident: e_1_2_11_23_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.04.003
– ident: e_1_2_11_32_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0149703
– ident: e_1_2_11_11_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003
– ident: e_1_2_11_5_1
  doi: 10.1017/S1461145710001690
SSID ssj0008645
Score 2.502215
Snippet Studies using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) typically compare an active protocol relative to a shorter sham (placebo) protocol. Both protocols...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
crossref
wiley
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage 3380
SubjectTerms Adult
Awareness
Cognitive Neuroscience
Cortex (motor)
Double-Blind Method
Electrical stimulation of the brain
Electrical stimuli
ESB
Female
Humans
Male
Motor Cortex
Perception
placebo
primary motor cortex
Protocol
reaction time
Reaction time task
Research Report
Scalp
sham
tDCS
Time Factors
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation - adverse effects
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation - methods
Young Adult
Title The time course of ineffective sham‐blinding during low‐intensity (1 mA) transcranial direct current stimulation
URI https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2Fejn.14497
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31228880
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2310184779
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2245627135
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC6899874
Volume 50
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3datRAFD7U3uiNP60_0VpGEakXKclkkuzg1VJbSsEiYmEvhDAzmdDV3aR0s0i98hF8BJ_FR_FJPGcmiV2rIN4sy-aEJLPn5zuZc74D8MxIovHKVcgVpqvCZFEoy4y4CG0UaYMxnlPv8Ovj7PBEHE3SyRq87HthPD_E8MKNLMP5azJwpReXjNx-qGlnUlIneZxkxJv_6u0v6qhR5gYUE51aOIqzSccqRFU8w5mrsegKwLxaJ3kZv7oAdHAL3ve37utOPu4uW71rPv_G6vifz3YbbnbAlI29Jt2BNVtvwOa4xqR8fsGeM1cq6t7Bb8D1vX5M3CYsUdEYTahnpqGCENZUDK_vykTQk7LFqZr_-PJVz6auf4b5vkg2az7hr1NfQN9esJ34-7f5-AVrKXYa_EC7YD7eMuM5pBh6o3k3bewunBzsv9s7DLtZDqERIslDREE8VUbYpORSYY5SmlzHaakQfxppUovOhpdxVdq8sqNIZxlx66G8SmWiK5Pcg_W6qe0DYNIoR8qjK64EZVxCazxHiMgjpgB2-n-1MB3ROc3bmBV9woPLW7jlDeDpIHrm2T3-JLTVq0bRGfiiIFiMyXGeywCeDIdx6Wm_RdW2WaIMbSpzmoEYwH2vScNVkpjzEfrOAPIVHRsEiPZ79Ug9PXX03xmlyLnAx3Qq9PcbL_aPjt2Xh_8u-ghuICSUvlxxC9bb86V9jLCr1dtwjYs3287KfgKx8C6h
linkProvider Wiley-Blackwell
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3NbtQwEB6VciiXAi0_gQIGIVQOqfLjJGuJy6pqtZR2D6iV9oIi23HUhd2kolmhcuIReASehUfhSZixk9ClICEuq9VmonWc-fnGHn8D8FwLovHKpB9JTFe5TgNfFClxEZogUBpjfERnh4_G6eiEH0ySyQq86s7COH6IfsGNLMP6azJwWpC-ZOXmfUVbkyK7Btft_hxBore_yKMGqW1RTIRq_iBMJy2vENXx9LcuR6MrEPNqpeRlBGtD0P5NeNcN3lWefNhZNGpHf_6N1_F_n-4WrLfYlA2dMt2GFVNtwOawwrx8fsFeMFstapfhN2Btt-sUtwkL1DVGTeqZrqkmhNUlwwHYShF0puz8VM5_fPmqZlN7hIa5o5FsVn_CX6euhr65YNvh92_z4UvWUPjU-IGmwVzIZdrRSDF0SPO24dgdONnfO94d-W07B19zHmc-AqEokZqbuIiExDSl0JkKk0IiBNVCJwb9TVSEZWGy0gwClaZEr4fyMhGxKnV8F1arujL3gQktLS-PKiPJKeniSuE9nAcONHmw3b3WXLdc59RyY5Z3OQ9Ob26n14NnveiZI_j4k9BWpxt5a-PnOSFjzI-zTHjwtL-MU09bLrIy9QJlaF85ojaIHtxzqtT_SxxG0QDdpwfZkpL1AsT8vXylmp5aBvCUsuSM42NaHfr7wPO9g7H98uDfRZ_A2uj46DA_fD1-8xBuIEIUrnpxC1abjwvzCFFYox5bY_sJTvwx6A
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwjV3NbtNAEB6VIkEvCFoohgILQqgcLNnrtTcrTlFpVApEPVApN2u9XqtBiV21iVBvPEIfoc_Co_RJOrNrW40KEpcoiseK7fn7xjv7DcB7o4jGS-qQayxXhcmiUJUZcRHaKCoM5nhOe4e_j7ODY3E4SSdr8KnbC-P5IfoXbuQZLl6Tg5-W1S0ntz9rWplU8h7cF2h3ZN5cHPVheJC5CcXEpxYO4mzS0gpRG09_6moyuoMw7zZK3gawLgONHsOjFjqyodf1E1iz9SZsDWssm-cX7ANzzZzuLfkmPNzrBrltwRJNgdEMeWYaatlgTcXw7l0jB8Y6dn6i59e_L4vZ1O1wYX7nIps1v_DXqW9xX1yw3fjP1Xz4kS0ouxn8QMtlPiMy41meGMaLeTsP7Ckcj_Z_7B2E7bSF0AiRyBBxCk-1ETYpudJYRZRGFnFaakSIRpnUYjjgZVyVVlZ2EBVZRux3KK9TlRSVSZ7Bet3U9jkwZbSjzSkqrgXVRKIo8BwhIo9pAtjtHntuWipymogxy7uSBDWUOw0F8K4XPfX8G38T2ul0l7cueJ4TcMXyVUoVwNv-MD56WhHRtW2WKEPLvpymFAaw7VXd_0sScz7A6BaAXDGCXoCIuVeP1NMTR9CdURErBd6mM5d_X3i-fzh2X178v-gbeHD0eZR_-zL--hI2EL8p31u4A-uLs6V9hRhpUbx2vnADADcQXw
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=The+time+course+of+ineffective+sham-blinding+during+low-intensity+%281+mA%29+transcranial+direct+current+stimulation&rft.jtitle=The+European+journal+of+neuroscience&rft.au=Greinacher%2C+Robert&rft.au=Buh%C3%B4t%2C+Larissa&rft.au=M%C3%B6ller%2C+Lisa&rft.au=Learmonth%2C+Gemma&rft.date=2019-10-01&rft.issn=1460-9568&rft.eissn=1460-9568&rft.volume=50&rft.issue=8&rft.spage=3380&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2Fejn.14497&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0953-816X&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0953-816X&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0953-816X&client=summon