Probing the depth of infants’ theory of mind: disunity in performance across paradigms
There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the ot...
Saved in:
Published in | Developmental science Vol. 21; no. 4; pp. e12600 - n/a |
---|---|
Main Authors | , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
England
Wiley-Blackwell
01.07.2018
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1363-755X 1467-7687 1467-7687 |
DOI | 10.1111/desc.12600 |
Cover
Abstract | There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous‐responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants’ performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within‐subject design that involved testing 18‐month‐old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation‐of‐ expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within‐subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants’ performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be
Most published articles on infant ToM have argued for a relatively sophisticated understanding of mental states in infants. To understand the depth of infants’ theory of mind, we compared 18‐month‐olds’ performance across pairs of tasks that measured the same concept. Unlike what has been reported in older children with elicited‐response tasks, no convergence in performance across tasks was apparent. |
---|---|
AbstractList | There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous-responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants' performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within-subject design that involved testing 18-month-old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation-of- expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within-subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants' performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be. There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous‐responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants’ performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within‐subject design that involved testing 18‐month‐old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation‐of‐ expectation ( VOE ) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within‐subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants’ performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous‐responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants’ performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within‐subject design that involved testing 18‐month‐old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation‐of‐ expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within‐subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants’ performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be Most published articles on infant ToM have argued for a relatively sophisticated understanding of mental states in infants. To understand the depth of infants’ theory of mind, we compared 18‐month‐olds’ performance across pairs of tasks that measured the same concept. Unlike what has been reported in older children with elicited‐response tasks, no convergence in performance across tasks was apparent. There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true ToM understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous-responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants’ performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within-subject design that involved testing 18-month-old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation-of-expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within-subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants’ performance on implicit ToM tasks. There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous‐responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants’ performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within‐subject design that involved testing 18‐month‐old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation‐of‐ expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within‐subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants’ performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous-responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants' performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within-subject design that involved testing 18-month-old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation-of- expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within-subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants' performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be.There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous-responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants' performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within-subject design that involved testing 18-month-old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation-of- expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within-subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants' performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be. |
Author | Yott, Jessica Poulin‐Dubois, Diane |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Diane surname: Poulin‐Dubois fullname: Poulin‐Dubois, Diane email: diane.poulindubois@concordia.ca organization: Concordia University – sequence: 2 givenname: Jessica surname: Yott fullname: Yott, Jessica organization: Concordia University |
BackLink | http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1183141$$DView record in ERIC https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28952180$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kV2L1TAQhoOsuB96471S8EaErknTpKkXghyPXywoqLB3IU0m52Rpk5q0yrnzb_j3_CW2p-tBFzE3E2aeeZl55xQd-eABofsEn5PpPTWQ9DkpOMa30AkpeZVXXFRH059ymleMXR6j05SuMMYlxeQOOi5EzQoi8Am6_BBD4_wmG7aQGeiHbRZs5rxVfkg_v_-Y8yHu5mTnvHmWGZdG74bdxGQ9RBtip7yGTOkYUsp6FZVxmy7dRbetahPcu45n6POr9afVm_zi_eu3qxcXuS5piXNVMI0ZNEWlmOC0aipb2gZjTWtTc0GZMkBqDEWhsMG0VMDBcqqpoZZzW9Ez9HzR7cemA6PBD1G1so-uU3Eng3Ly74p3W7kJXyUTFWElmQQeXwvE8GWENMjOJQ1tqzyEMUlSl5SXbDJuQh_dQK_CGP20niwwE4xRImbq4Z8THUb5bfoEPFgAiE4fyut3hAhK9hM9Wep7SyPYA0OwnC8u54vL_cUnGN-AtRvU4MK8rWv_3UKWlm-uhd1_xOXL9cfV0vMLuIm91A |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cobeha_2024_101480 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11229_024_04809_3 crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12955 crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_13064 crossref_primary_10_1111_cdep_12483 crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_2359 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_191998 crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_13224 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0295221 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_190068 crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_13343 crossref_primary_10_1007_s11229_020_02655_7 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2019_101350 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_12891 crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0241721 crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12904 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2018_07_012 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2019_06_009 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyt_2024_1189777 crossref_primary_10_1038_s44159_022_00037_z crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2024_1455941 crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_2348 crossref_primary_10_1111_infa_12277 crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1551 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2022_101273 crossref_primary_10_1080_15248372_2018_1544140 crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797617747090 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2022_105419 crossref_primary_10_1111_infa_12358 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2018_09_005 crossref_primary_10_1080_15475441_2020_1820339 crossref_primary_10_2478_plc_2019_0006 crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_211278 crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_13955 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2023_1142302 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02063.x 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.008 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.618 10.1111/cdev.12311 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.04.004 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.07.008 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.08.003 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00946.x 10.1093/scan/nsu081 10.1111/desc.12148 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00719 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.001 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00736.x 10.1080/15248372.2011.608198 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.002 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199334919.001.0001 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_5 10.4324/9781315807874 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00152.x 10.1017/S0305000904006270 10.1016/S0065-2407(09)03702-1 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.09.001 10.1111/cdep.12183 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838 10.1111/1467-8624.00333 10.1016/j.psfr.2013.11.002 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x 10.1126/science.1107621 10.1111/mila.12036 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00347.x 10.1177/0963721416673174 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00980.x 10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12 10.1177/0956797611411584 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115033 10.1111/desc.12173 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.014 10.1177/0956797614558717 10.1097/PEP.0b013e31829db85b 10.1017/S0142716400001053 10.1111/1467-8624.00304 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00813.x 10.1016/j.dr.2014.04.001 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00105.x 10.1080/15248372.2012.689390 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01098.x 10.1126/science.1190792 10.1111/cdev.12533 10.1037/a0016923 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00478.x 10.1371/journal.pone.0142405 10.1037/14341-003 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.11.005 10.2307/1130386 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02046.x 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.06.005 10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4 10.1080/15248372.2015.1086771 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02060.x |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd 2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Copyright © 2018 John Wiley & Sons Ltd |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd – notice: 2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. – notice: Copyright © 2018 John Wiley & Sons Ltd |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO ERI PET REK WWN CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QP 7TK 7X8 5PM |
DOI | 10.1111/desc.12600 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef ERIC ERIC (Ovid) ERIC ERIC ERIC (Legacy Platform) ERIC( SilverPlatter ) ERIC ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform) Education Resources Information Center (ERIC) ERIC Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic PubMed Central (Full Participant titles) |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef ERIC MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE CrossRef Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts ERIC MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: ERI name: ERIC url: https://eric.ed.gov/ sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1467-7687 |
ERIC | EJ1183141 |
EndPage | n/a |
ExternalDocumentID | PMC5871541 28952180 EJ1183141 10_1111_desc_12600 DESC12600 |
Genre | article Journal Article |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) funderid: #435‐2012‐1403 – fundername: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) funderid: #R01HD068458 – fundername: NICHD NIH HHS grantid: R01 HD068458 |
GroupedDBID | --- .3N .GA .Y3 05W 0R~ 10A 1OC 29F 31~ 33P 36B 4.4 50Y 50Z 51W 51Y 52M 52O 52Q 52S 52T 52U 52V 52W 53G 5GY 5HH 5LA 5VS 66C 702 7PT 8-0 8-1 8-3 8-4 8-5 8UM 930 A01 A04 AABNI AAESR AAHHS AAHQN AAHSB AAIPD AAMNL AANHP AAONW AAOUF AASGY AAXRX AAYCA AAZKR ABCQN ABCUV ABDBF ABEML ABIVO ABPVW ABQWH ABSOO ABXGK ACAHQ ACBKW ACBWZ ACCFJ ACCZN ACFBH ACGFS ACGOF ACHQT ACMXC ACPOU ACRPL ACSCC ACUHS ACXQS ACYXJ ADBBV ADBTR ADEMA ADEOM ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADNMO ADXAS ADZMN ADZOD AEEZP AEIGN AEIMD AEQDE AEUQT AEUYR AFBPY AFEBI AFFNX AFFPM AFGKR AFKFF AFPWT AFWVQ AFYRF AFZJQ AHBTC AIACR AIFKG AIURR AIWBW AJBDE ALAGY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQN ALVPJ AMBMR AMYDB ASPBG ASTYK AVWKF AZBYB AZFZN AZVAB BAFTC BDRZF BFHJK BMXJE BNVMJ BQESF BROTX BRXPI BY8 CAG COF CS3 D-7 D-C D-D DCZOG DPXWK DR2 DRFUL DRMAN DRSSH EAD EAP EBD EBS EJD EMB EMK EMOBN EPS EST ESX F00 F01 F5P FEDTE FUBAC G-S G.N G50 GODZA HGLYW HVGLF HZI HZ~ IHE IX1 J0M K48 KBYEO LATKE LC2 LC4 LEEKS LH4 LITHE LOXES LP6 LP7 LUTES LW6 LYRES MEWTI MK4 MRFUL MRMAN MRSSH MSFUL MSMAN MSSSH MXFUL MXMAN MXSSH N04 N06 N9A NF~ O66 O9- OIG OVD P2P P2W P2Y P4C PQQKQ Q.N Q11 QB0 R.K ROL RX1 SUPJJ SV3 TEORI TUS UB1 UPT UQL W8V W99 WBKPD WHDPE WIH WII WIJ WOHZO WQZ WRC WSUWO WXSBR XG1 ZZTAW ~IA ~WP AAMMB AAYXX AEFGJ AEYWJ AGHNM AGQPQ AGXDD AIDQK AIDYY CITATION 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO ERI PET REK WWN CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QP 7TK 7X8 5PM |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c4340-a25c05eb27a58637b7f4fb00c39d96835ade190e22a0d034ae6ef63c3d3f66f73 |
IEDL.DBID | DR2 |
ISSN | 1363-755X 1467-7687 |
IngestDate | Thu Aug 21 14:22:05 EDT 2025 Thu Jul 10 16:34:38 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 14 20:43:49 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 05:58:19 EDT 2025 Tue Sep 02 20:00:15 EDT 2025 Wed Oct 01 03:30:54 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:01:25 EDT 2025 Wed Jan 22 16:59:13 EST 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | false |
IsPeerReviewed | false |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 4 |
Language | English |
License | http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/termsAndConditions#vor 2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4340-a25c05eb27a58637b7f4fb00c39d96835ade190e22a0d034ae6ef63c3d3f66f73 |
Notes | This research was supported by graduate fellowships from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) to Jessica Yott and research grants from SSHRC (#435‐2012‐1403) and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD; #R01HD068458) to Diane Poulin‐Dubois. Funding Information ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
PMID | 28952180 |
PQID | 2058553181 |
PQPubID | 2030115 |
PageCount | 11 |
ParticipantIDs | pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_5871541 proquest_miscellaneous_1943645301 proquest_journals_2058553181 pubmed_primary_28952180 eric_primary_EJ1183141 crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12600 crossref_citationtrail_10_1111_desc_12600 wiley_primary_10_1111_desc_12600_DESC12600 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | July 2018 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2018-07-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 07 year: 2018 text: July 2018 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | England |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: England – name: Oxford |
PublicationTitle | Developmental science |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Dev Sci |
PublicationYear | 2018 |
Publisher | Wiley-Blackwell Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
Publisher_xml | – name: Wiley-Blackwell – name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
References | 1995; 31 2015; 39 2013; 25 2010; 14 2013; 28 2010; 13 2004; 7 2015; 142 2008; 109 2009; 112 2011; 14 2005; 28 2009; 116 2010; 61 2009; 12 2004; 31 2013; 14 2015; 82 2016; 87 2014; 59 2011; 22 2005; 308 2014; 17 2007; 18 2001; 72 2015; 6 2004; 40 2017; 26 2010 2000; 21 1988; 59 2006; 9 2015; 10 2016; 10 1999; 23 2008; 11 2016; 17 2011; 5 2012; 30 1998; 69 1999 2014; 86 1994; 9 2015; 26 2012a; 17 2009; 32 1997; 33 2005; 9 2010; 330 2017 2005; 98 2015 2008; 44 2014 2012b; 15 2012; 6 2014; 34 2009; 37 2016; 67 e_1_2_7_5_1 e_1_2_7_3_1 Trudeau N. (e_1_2_7_64_1) 1999; 23 e_1_2_7_9_1 e_1_2_7_7_1 e_1_2_7_19_1 e_1_2_7_60_1 e_1_2_7_62_1 e_1_2_7_15_1 Mangold P. (e_1_2_7_30_1) 2010 e_1_2_7_41_1 e_1_2_7_13_1 e_1_2_7_43_1 e_1_2_7_66_1 e_1_2_7_11_1 e_1_2_7_45_1 e_1_2_7_68_1 e_1_2_7_47_1 e_1_2_7_26_1 e_1_2_7_49_1 Poulin‐Dubois D. (e_1_2_7_38_1) 1999 Colombo J. (e_1_2_7_17_1) 2014 e_1_2_7_73_1 e_1_2_7_50_1 e_1_2_7_71_1 e_1_2_7_25_1 e_1_2_7_31_1 e_1_2_7_52_1 e_1_2_7_23_1 e_1_2_7_33_1 e_1_2_7_54_1 e_1_2_7_21_1 e_1_2_7_56_1 e_1_2_7_37_1 e_1_2_7_58_1 e_1_2_7_39_1 e_1_2_7_6_1 e_1_2_7_4_1 e_1_2_7_8_1 e_1_2_7_18_1 e_1_2_7_16_1 e_1_2_7_40_1 e_1_2_7_61_1 e_1_2_7_2_1 e_1_2_7_14_1 e_1_2_7_63_1 e_1_2_7_12_1 e_1_2_7_44_1 e_1_2_7_65_1 e_1_2_7_10_1 e_1_2_7_46_1 e_1_2_7_67_1 e_1_2_7_48_1 e_1_2_7_69_1 e_1_2_7_27_1 e_1_2_7_29_1 e_1_2_7_72_1 Powell L. (e_1_2_7_42_1) 2017 e_1_2_7_51_1 e_1_2_7_70_1 e_1_2_7_53_1 e_1_2_7_24_1 e_1_2_7_32_1 e_1_2_7_55_1 e_1_2_7_22_1 Kulke L. (e_1_2_7_28_1) 2017 e_1_2_7_34_1 Perner J. (e_1_2_7_35_1) 2010 e_1_2_7_57_1 e_1_2_7_20_1 e_1_2_7_36_1 e_1_2_7_59_1 |
References_xml | – start-page: 241 year: 2010 end-page: 261 – volume: 9 start-page: 377 year: 1994 end-page: 395 article-title: Implicit understanding of belief publication-title: Cognitive Development – volume: 9 start-page: 182 year: 2006 end-page: 188 article-title: Change in children's understanding of others’ intentional actions publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 7 start-page: 283 year: 2004 end-page: 288 article-title: Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 59 start-page: 59 year: 2014 end-page: 69 article-title: Executive functions and theory of mind understanding in young children: A reciprocal relation? publication-title: Psychologie Française – volume: 30 start-page: 75 year: 2012 end-page: 86 article-title: Direct and indirect measures of level‐2 perspective‐taking in children and adults publication-title: British Journal of Developmental Psychology – volume: 9 start-page: 462 year: 2005 end-page: 463 article-title: Do infants really understand false belief? publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences – volume: 59 start-page: 26 year: 1988 end-page: 37 article-title: Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance–reality distinction publication-title: Child Development – volume: 10 start-page: 184 year: 2016 end-page: 189 article-title: Cognitive architecture of belief reasoning in children and adults: A primer on the two systems account publication-title: Child Development Perspectives – volume: 17 start-page: 647 year: 2014 end-page: 659 article-title: False belief in infancy: A fresh look publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 61 start-page: 366 year: 2010 end-page: 395 article-title: Attributing false beliefs about non‐obvious properties at 18 months publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 25 start-page: 395 year: 2013 end-page: 401 article-title: Concurrent validity of the TIMP and the Bayley III scales at 6 weeks corrected age publication-title: Pediatric Physical Therapy – year: 2014 – volume: 26 start-page: 68 year: 2017 end-page: 74 article-title: The developmental origins of false‐belief understanding publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science – volume: 308 start-page: 255 year: 2005 end-page: 258 article-title: Do 15‐month‐old infants understand false beliefs? publication-title: Science – start-page: 193 year: 2014 end-page: 229 – volume: 6 start-page: 719 year: 2015 article-title: Developmental pathways for social understanding: Linking social cognition to social contexts publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology – volume: 15 start-page: 113 year: 2012b end-page: 122 article-title: Eighteen‐ and 24‐month‐old infants correct others in anticipation of action mistakes publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 11 start-page: 862 year: 2008 end-page: 868 article-title: The link between infant attention to goal‐directed action and later theory of mind abilities publication-title: Developmental Science – start-page: 257 year: 1999 end-page: 280 – volume: 109 start-page: 295 year: 2008 end-page: 315 article-title: Can an agent's false belief be corrected by an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18‐month‐old infants publication-title: Cognition – volume: 69 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 1998 end-page: 34 article-title: Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach publication-title: Cognition – start-page: 79 year: 2015 end-page: 150 – volume: 30 start-page: 30 year: 2012 end-page: 44 article-title: Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to a geometric shape at 17 months publication-title: British Journal of Developmental Psychology – volume: 17 start-page: 660 year: 2014 end-page: 664 article-title: How fresh a look? A reply to Heyes publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 14 start-page: 110 year: 2010 end-page: 118 article-title: False‐belief understanding in infants publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences – volume: 28 start-page: 645 year: 2005 end-page: 668 article-title: Executive function and theory of mind in 2 year olds: A family affair? publication-title: Developmental Neuropsychology – volume: 23 start-page: 61 year: 1999 end-page: 73 article-title: Une adaptation en français québécois du MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory [A Quebec‐French adaptation of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory] publication-title: La revue d'orthophonie et d'audiologie – volume: 330 start-page: 1830 year: 2010 end-page: 1834 article-title: The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults publication-title: Science – volume: 28 start-page: 606 year: 2013 end-page: 637 article-title: How to construct a minimal theory of mind publication-title: Mind & Language – volume: 14 start-page: 292 year: 2011 end-page: 305 article-title: False‐belief understanding in 2.5‐year‐olds: Evidence from violation of expectation change of location and unexpected‐contents tasks publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 39 start-page: 63 year: 2015 end-page: 72 article-title: How to help: Can more active behavioral measures help transcend the infant false belief debate? publication-title: New Ideas in Psychology – volume: 18 start-page: 587 year: 2007 end-page: 592 article-title: Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2‐year‐olds publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 98 start-page: 137 year: 2005 end-page: 155 article-title: Infants’ understanding of object‐directed action publication-title: Cognition – volume: 26 start-page: 1353 year: 2015 end-page: 1367 article-title: A second look at automatic theory of mind: Reconsidering Kovács, Téglás, and Endress (2010) publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 14 start-page: 87 year: 2013 end-page: 99 article-title: Is false belief skin‐deep? The agent's eye status influences infants’ reasoning in belief‐inducing situations publication-title: Journal of Cognition and Development – volume: 5 start-page: 39 year: 2011 end-page: 43 article-title: Theory of mind in infancy publication-title: Child Development Perspectives – volume: 31 start-page: 587 year: 2004 end-page: 608 article-title: Measuring productive vocabulary of toddlers in low‐income families: Concurrent and predictive validity of three sources of data publication-title: Journal of Child Language – volume: 32 start-page: 404 year: 2009 end-page: 415 article-title: Infants’ understanding of intention from 10 to 14 months: Interrelations among violation of expectancy and imitation tasks publication-title: Infant Behavior and Development – volume: 33 start-page: 12 year: 1997 end-page: 21 article-title: Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14‐ and 18‐month‐olds publication-title: Developmental Psychology – volume: 86 start-page: 486 year: 2014 end-page: 502 article-title: Explicit theory of mind is even more unified than previously assumed: Belief ascription and understanding aspectuality emerge together in development publication-title: Child Development – volume: 87 start-page: 1221 year: 2016 end-page: 1232 article-title: Understanding of goals, beliefs, and desires predicts morally relevant theory of mind: A longitudinal investigation publication-title: Child Development – volume: 14 start-page: 480 year: 2013 end-page: 498 article-title: Concurrent relations between perspective‐taking skills, desire understanding, and internal‐state vocabulary publication-title: Journal of Cognition and Development – volume: 72 start-page: 1032 year: 2001 end-page: 1053 article-title: Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind publication-title: Child Development – volume: 40 start-page: 1105 year: 2004 end-page: 1122 article-title: Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3 publication-title: Developmental Psychology – volume: 17 start-page: 672 year: 2012a end-page: 691 article-title: 18‐month‐olds predict specific action mistakes through attribution of false belief, not ignorance, and intervene accordingly publication-title: Infancy – volume: 12 start-page: 746 year: 2009 end-page: 752 article-title: Continuity in social cognition from infancy to childhood publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 6 start-page: 184 year: 2012 end-page: 192 article-title: Modified checklist for autism in toddlers (M‐CHAT) screening at 18 months of age predicts concurrent understanding of desires, word learning and expressive vocabulary publication-title: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders – volume: 30 start-page: 156 year: 2012 end-page: 171 article-title: Breaking the rules: Do infants have a true understanding of false belief publication-title: British Journal of Developmental Psychology – year: 2010 – volume: 10 start-page: e0142405 year: 2015 article-title: Processing demands impact 3‐year‐olds’ performance in a spontaneous‐response task: New evidence for the processing‐load account of early false‐belief understanding publication-title: PLoS ONE – volume: 22 start-page: 878 year: 2011 end-page: 880 article-title: Do 18‐month‐olds really attribute mental states to others? A critical test publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 10 start-page: 327 year: 2015 end-page: 334 article-title: Spontaneous mentalizing captures variability in the cortical thickness of social brain regions publication-title: Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience – volume: 44 start-page: 618 year: 2008 end-page: 623 article-title: Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind publication-title: Developmental Psychology – volume: 31 start-page: 838 year: 1995 end-page: 850 article-title: Understanding the intentions of others: Re‐enactment of intended acts by 18‐month‐old children publication-title: Developmental Psychology – volume: 112 start-page: 337 year: 2009 end-page: 342 article-title: Eighteen‐month‐old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm publication-title: Cognition – volume: 67 start-page: 159 year: 2016 end-page: 186 article-title: Psychological reasoning in infancy publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology – volume: 13 start-page: 907 year: 2010 end-page: 912 article-title: Seventeen‐month‐olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication publication-title: Developmental Science – volume: 30 start-page: 172 year: 2012 end-page: 187 article-title: Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age publication-title: British Journal of Developmental Psychology – volume: 82 start-page: 32 year: 2015 end-page: 56 article-title: Infants understand deceptive intentions to implant false beliefs about identity: New evidence for early mentalistic reasoning publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 17 start-page: 683 year: 2016 end-page: 698 article-title: Are infants’ theory of mind abilities well integrated? Implicit understanding of intentions, desires, and beliefs publication-title: Journal of Cognition and Development – year: 2017 – volume: 21 start-page: 95 year: 2000 end-page: 115 article-title: Short‐form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories publication-title: Applied Psycholinguistics – volume: 37 start-page: 55 year: 2009 end-page: 104 article-title: The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy publication-title: Advances in Child Development and Behavior – volume: 34 start-page: 265 year: 2014 end-page: 293 article-title: To belief or not belief: Children's theory of mind publication-title: Developmental Review – volume: 142 start-page: 44 year: 2015 end-page: 59 article-title: Goal attributions and instrumental helping at 14 and 24 months of age publication-title: Cognition – volume: 18 start-page: 580 year: 2007 end-page: 586 article-title: Attribution of beliefs by 13‐month‐old infants publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 116 start-page: 953 year: 2009 end-page: 970 article-title: Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief‐like states? publication-title: Psychological Review – volume: 72 start-page: 655 year: 2001 end-page: 684 article-title: Meta‐analysis of theory‐of‐mind development: The truth about false belief publication-title: Child Development – ident: e_1_2_7_6_1 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006 – ident: e_1_2_7_13_1 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105 – ident: e_1_2_7_62_1 doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02063.x – ident: e_1_2_7_63_1 doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x – ident: e_1_2_7_57_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.008 – ident: e_1_2_7_67_1 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.618 – ident: e_1_2_7_44_1 doi: 10.1111/cdev.12311 – ident: e_1_2_7_70_1 doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.04.004 – ident: e_1_2_7_2_1 doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.07.008 – ident: e_1_2_7_52_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.08.003 – ident: e_1_2_7_58_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00946.x – ident: e_1_2_7_46_1 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu081 – ident: e_1_2_7_22_1 doi: 10.1111/desc.12148 – ident: e_1_2_7_9_1 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00719 – ident: e_1_2_7_48_1 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.001 – ident: e_1_2_7_4_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00736.x – ident: e_1_2_7_40_1 doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.608198 – ident: e_1_2_7_43_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.002 – ident: e_1_2_7_65_1 doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199334919.001.0001 – start-page: 257 volume-title: Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life year: 1999 ident: e_1_2_7_38_1 – ident: e_1_2_7_10_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006 – ident: e_1_2_7_24_1 doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_5 – volume: 23 start-page: 61 year: 1999 ident: e_1_2_7_64_1 article-title: Une adaptation en français québécois du MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory [A Quebec‐French adaptation of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory] publication-title: La revue d'orthophonie et d'audiologie – start-page: 193 volume-title: Individual differences in infancy: Reliability, stability, and prediction year: 2014 ident: e_1_2_7_17_1 doi: 10.4324/9781315807874 – ident: e_1_2_7_55_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00152.x – ident: e_1_2_7_34_1 doi: 10.1017/S0305000904006270 – ident: e_1_2_7_39_1 doi: 10.1016/S0065-2407(09)03702-1 – ident: e_1_2_7_51_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.09.001 – ident: e_1_2_7_29_1 doi: 10.1111/cdep.12183 – ident: e_1_2_7_31_1 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838 – start-page: 241 volume-title: Cognition and neuropsychology: International perspectives on psychological science year: 2010 ident: e_1_2_7_35_1 – ident: e_1_2_7_14_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00333 – volume-title: Interact user guide, V. 8.0 year: 2010 ident: e_1_2_7_30_1 – ident: e_1_2_7_41_1 doi: 10.1016/j.psfr.2013.11.002 – ident: e_1_2_7_59_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x – volume-title: Are implicit theory of mind findings robust? Some doubts from converging non‐replications across the lifespan year: 2017 ident: e_1_2_7_42_1 – ident: e_1_2_7_60_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x – ident: e_1_2_7_33_1 doi: 10.1126/science.1107621 – ident: e_1_2_7_11_1 doi: 10.1111/mila.12036 – ident: e_1_2_7_68_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00347.x – ident: e_1_2_7_49_1 doi: 10.1177/0963721416673174 – ident: e_1_2_7_21_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00980.x – ident: e_1_2_7_45_1 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12 – ident: e_1_2_7_54_1 doi: 10.1177/0956797611411584 – ident: e_1_2_7_69_1 doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4 – ident: e_1_2_7_5_1 doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115033 – ident: e_1_2_7_50_1 doi: 10.1111/desc.12173 – ident: e_1_2_7_23_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.014 – ident: e_1_2_7_37_1 doi: 10.1177/0956797614558717 – ident: e_1_2_7_12_1 doi: 10.1097/PEP.0b013e31829db85b – ident: e_1_2_7_19_1 doi: 10.1017/S0142716400001053 – ident: e_1_2_7_66_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00304 – ident: e_1_2_7_71_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00813.x – ident: e_1_2_7_47_1 doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.04.001 – ident: e_1_2_7_25_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00105.x – ident: e_1_2_7_15_1 doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.689390 – ident: e_1_2_7_26_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01098.x – ident: e_1_2_7_27_1 doi: 10.1126/science.1190792 – volume-title: Are implicit theory of mind findings robust? Some doubts from converging non‐replications across the lifespan year: 2017 ident: e_1_2_7_28_1 – ident: e_1_2_7_56_1 doi: 10.1111/cdev.12533 – ident: e_1_2_7_3_1 doi: 10.1037/a0016923 – ident: e_1_2_7_8_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00478.x – ident: e_1_2_7_53_1 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142405 – ident: e_1_2_7_7_1 doi: 10.1037/14341-003 – ident: e_1_2_7_36_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.11.005 – ident: e_1_2_7_20_1 doi: 10.2307/1130386 – ident: e_1_2_7_61_1 doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02046.x – ident: e_1_2_7_32_1 doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.06.005 – ident: e_1_2_7_18_1 – ident: e_1_2_7_16_1 doi: 10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4 – ident: e_1_2_7_73_1 doi: 10.1080/15248372.2015.1086771 – ident: e_1_2_7_72_1 doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02060.x |
SSID | ssj0004301 |
Score | 2.4556956 |
Snippet | There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic... There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true ToM understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants... |
SourceID | pubmedcentral proquest pubmed eric crossref wiley |
SourceType | Open Access Repository Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | e12600 |
SubjectTerms | Aptitude Attention Beliefs Child Children Cognition Cognitive Ability Comparative Analysis Comprehension Correlation Female Humans Infant Infants Intention Interaction Male Motivation Psychological Patterns Reproducibility of Results Social Perception Task Analysis Task Performance and Analysis Theory of Mind Theory of Mind - physiology Validity |
Title | Probing the depth of infants’ theory of mind: disunity in performance across paradigms |
URI | https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2Fdesc.12600 http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1183141 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28952180 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2058553181 https://www.proquest.com/docview/1943645301 https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC5871541 |
Volume | 21 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
journalDatabaseRights | – providerCode: PRVEBS databaseName: Academic Search Ultimate customDbUrl: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?authtype=ip,shib&custid=s3936755&profile=ehost&defaultdb=asn eissn: 1467-7687 dateEnd: 20241001 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0004301 issn: 1363-755X databaseCode: ABDBF dateStart: 19980401 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://search.ebscohost.com/direct.asp?db=asn providerName: EBSCOhost – providerCode: PRVWIB databaseName: Wiley Online Library - Core collection (SURFmarket) issn: 1363-755X databaseCode: DR2 dateStart: 19980101 customDbUrl: isFulltext: true eissn: 1467-7687 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: false ssIdentifier: ssj0004301 providerName: Wiley-Blackwell |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9QwELaqnnoBSikstJURXEDKyo6fqbigdquqEggBlfaCIsd22hU0u2p2D8uJv8Hf45cwdh67CwgJblE8ediemXxjz3xB6Hnwi1R7lUCwnCbcO5sUlmvQ5VQ67TxhRShwfvNWnl_yi7EYb6FXXS1Mww_RL7gFy4j-Ohi4Keo1I3e-tkMa-NXBAVMm4h7t-xV3FGekLbpiiRJi3HKThjSe1aUbX6PNhOc1pPl7wuQ6kI1forO76FPXhyYB5fNwMS-G9usv9I7_28l76E4LUfHrRqd20Zav7qOd3lMu99D4XWBvqq4woEfs_Gx-jaclBl0NSTU_vn3HsTxyGU7eQMx_jN2kXoDvWIIMnq1KFbCJw4ID_7ibXN3UD9Dl2ejjyXnS_qIhsZxxkphUWCIgOldGaMlUoUpegiVblrlMArozzgPk8GlqiCOMGy99KZlljpVSlorto-1qWvlHCGvLDaGGqIxYTqky0pYa7pmWzmeWsQF60U1Vblv-8vAbjS95F8eEwcrjYA3Qs1521rB2_FFqP8x4LzG6gGiLUU4H6KDTgbw16TpPCURW4LE0ND_tm8EYww6Lqfx0Uec042FbFzRvgB42KtPfHSJbgEoanqo2lKkXCETfmy3V5DoSfguIakV4rZdRV_7Spfx09OEkHj3-F-EnaAeAoG7SkA_Q9vx24Q8BbM2Lo2hUR3FZ4Se_Iybz |
linkProvider | Wiley-Blackwell |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1fb9MwELdgPLAX_g8KA4zgBaRUTuzYDm9o61TGNiHYpL5Fju1sFSytSPtQnvgafD0-CXdOmraAkOAtii9OYt9dfhff_UzIC_SLsfYqgmA5iYR3Niqs0KDLiXTaecYLLHA-PpHDM3E4Skdtbg7WwjT8EN0PN7SM4K_RwPGH9JqVO1_bfowE61fJNVygQ7vc_7BijxKctWVXPFJpOmrZSTGRZ3XtxvdoM-V5DWv-njK5DmXDt-jgZrPhah0oDDEF5VN_Piv69usvBI___Zq3yI0WpdI3jVrdJld8dYdsd85ycZeM3iOBU3VOAUBS56ezCzopKagr5tX8-PadhgrJBZ68hLD_NXXjeg7uYwEydLqqVqAmjAtFCnI3Pr-s75Gzg8Hp3jBqd2mIrOCCRSZJLUshQFcm1ZKrQpWiBGO2PHOZBIBnnAfU4ZPEMMe4MF76UnLLHS-lLBXfIVvVpPIPCNVWGBYbpjJmRRwrI22poc-kdD6znPfIy-Vc5balMMedND7ny1AGBysPg9UjzzvZaUPc8UepHZzyTmJwCAEXj0XcI7tLJchbq67zhEFwBU5LQ_OzrhnsERdZTOUn8zqPM4Eru6B6PXK_0ZmudwhuAS1puKva0KZOALm-N1uq8UXg_E4hsE3xsV4FZfnLK-X7g4974ejhvwg_JdeHp8dH-dHbk3ePyDbgQt1kJe-SrdmXuX8M2GtWPAkW9hMYaCoO |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9NAEB6VIqFeKK-WlAKL4AKSo7V3vV4jLqhNVApUFVApF2St99FGUCfCySGc-Bv8PX4Js34lAYQEtyg7cbzrmfH3eWc-AzzxeTGUNgmQLEcBt0YHueYSfTkSRhpLWe4bnN-eiKMzfjyKRxvwou2FqfUhugduPjKqfO0DfGrcSpAbW-p-6PXVr8BVLpBeeUj0bikexRltuq5YkMTxqBEn9XU8y9-u3Y7WK55XoObvFZOrSLa6FQ234WM7iboC5VN_Psv7-usv-o7_O8sbcL3BqORl7VQ3YcMWt2CrS5WL2zA69fJNxTlB-EiMnc4uyMQRdFZfVfPj23dS9Ucu_JeXSPqfEzMu55g8FmhDpsteBaKqZSFegNyMzy_LO3A2HHw4OAqadzQEmjNOAxXFmsZIzxMVS8GSPHHcYShrlppUILxTxiLmsFGkqKGMKyusE0wzw5wQLmE7sFlMCnsXiNRc0VDRJKWah2GihHYSjxk5Y1PNWA-etpcq042AuX-PxuesJTJ-sbJqsXrwuLOd1rIdf7Ta8Ve8sxgcI91iIQ97sN_6QNbEdJlFFKkVpiyJw4-6YYxGv8WiCjuZl1mYcr-vi57Xg93aZbqjI7VFrCTxX5M1Z-oMvNL3-kgxvqgUv2OktbE_rWeVr_xlStnh4P1B9WnvX4wfwrXTw2H25tXJ63uwhaBQ1iXJ-7A5-zK39xF4zfIHVXz9BIHnKL0 |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Probing+the+depth+of+infants%27+theory+of+mind%3A+disunity+in+performance+across+paradigms&rft.jtitle=Developmental+science&rft.au=Poulin-Dubois%2C+Diane&rft.au=Yott%2C+Jessica&rft.date=2018-07-01&rft.eissn=1467-7687&rft.volume=21&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=e12600&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2Fdesc.12600&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F28952180&rft.externalDocID=28952180 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon |