Probing the depth of infants’ theory of mind: disunity in performance across paradigms

There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the ot...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Published inDevelopmental science Vol. 21; no. 4; pp. e12600 - n/a
Main Authors Poulin‐Dubois, Diane, Yott, Jessica
Format Journal Article
LanguageEnglish
Published England Wiley-Blackwell 01.07.2018
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
Subjects
Online AccessGet full text
ISSN1363-755X
1467-7687
1467-7687
DOI10.1111/desc.12600

Cover

Abstract There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous‐responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants’ performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within‐subject design that involved testing 18‐month‐old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation‐of‐ expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within‐subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants’ performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be Most published articles on infant ToM have argued for a relatively sophisticated understanding of mental states in infants. To understand the depth of infants’ theory of mind, we compared 18‐month‐olds’ performance across pairs of tasks that measured the same concept. Unlike what has been reported in older children with elicited‐response tasks, no convergence in performance across tasks was apparent.
AbstractList There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous-responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants' performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within-subject design that involved testing 18-month-old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation-of- expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within-subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants' performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be.
There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous‐responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants’ performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within‐subject design that involved testing 18‐month‐old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation‐of‐ expectation ( VOE ) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within‐subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants’ performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be
There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous‐responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants’ performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within‐subject design that involved testing 18‐month‐old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation‐of‐ expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within‐subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants’ performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be Most published articles on infant ToM have argued for a relatively sophisticated understanding of mental states in infants. To understand the depth of infants’ theory of mind, we compared 18‐month‐olds’ performance across pairs of tasks that measured the same concept. Unlike what has been reported in older children with elicited‐response tasks, no convergence in performance across tasks was apparent.
There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true ToM understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous-responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants’ performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within-subject design that involved testing 18-month-old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation-of-expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within-subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants’ performance on implicit ToM tasks.
There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous‐responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants’ performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within‐subject design that involved testing 18‐month‐old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation‐of‐ expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within‐subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants’ performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be
There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous-responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants' performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within-subject design that involved testing 18-month-old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation-of- expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within-subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants' performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be.There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants possess the same false belief understanding that older children have but their competence is masked by task demands. On the other hand, others have proposed that preverbal infants are incapable of mental state attribution and simply respond to superficial features of the events in spontaneous-responses tasks. In the current study, we aimed to clarify the nature of infants' performance in tasks designed to assess implicit theory of mind (ToM) by adopting a within-subject design that involved testing 18-month-old infants on two batteries of tasks measuring the same four ToM constructs (intention, desire, true belief, and false belief). One battery included tasks based on the violation-of- expectation (VOE) procedure, whereas the other set of tasks was based on the interactive, helping procedure. Replication of the original findings varied across tasks, due to methodological changes and the use of a within-subject design. Convergent validity was examined by comparing performance on VOE and interactive tasks that are considered to be measures of the same theory of mind concept. The results revealed no significant relations between performance on the pairs of tasks for any of the four ToM constructs measured. This pattern of results is discussed in terms of current conflicting accounts of infants' performance on implicit ToM tasks. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U3vqfe_zdhA&feature=youtu.be.
Author Yott, Jessica
Poulin‐Dubois, Diane
Author_xml – sequence: 1
  givenname: Diane
  surname: Poulin‐Dubois
  fullname: Poulin‐Dubois, Diane
  email: diane.poulindubois@concordia.ca
  organization: Concordia University
– sequence: 2
  givenname: Jessica
  surname: Yott
  fullname: Yott, Jessica
  organization: Concordia University
BackLink http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1183141$$DView record in ERIC
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28952180$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed
BookMark eNp9kV2L1TAQhoOsuB96471S8EaErknTpKkXghyPXywoqLB3IU0m52Rpk5q0yrnzb_j3_CW2p-tBFzE3E2aeeZl55xQd-eABofsEn5PpPTWQ9DkpOMa30AkpeZVXXFRH059ymleMXR6j05SuMMYlxeQOOi5EzQoi8Am6_BBD4_wmG7aQGeiHbRZs5rxVfkg_v_-Y8yHu5mTnvHmWGZdG74bdxGQ9RBtip7yGTOkYUsp6FZVxmy7dRbetahPcu45n6POr9afVm_zi_eu3qxcXuS5piXNVMI0ZNEWlmOC0aipb2gZjTWtTc0GZMkBqDEWhsMG0VMDBcqqpoZZzW9Ez9HzR7cemA6PBD1G1so-uU3Eng3Ly74p3W7kJXyUTFWElmQQeXwvE8GWENMjOJQ1tqzyEMUlSl5SXbDJuQh_dQK_CGP20niwwE4xRImbq4Z8THUb5bfoEPFgAiE4fyut3hAhK9hM9Wep7SyPYA0OwnC8u54vL_cUnGN-AtRvU4MK8rWv_3UKWlm-uhd1_xOXL9cfV0vMLuIm91A
CitedBy_id crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cobeha_2024_101480
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11229_024_04809_3
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12955
crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_13064
crossref_primary_10_1111_cdep_12483
crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_2359
crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_191998
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_13224
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0295221
crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_190068
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_13343
crossref_primary_10_1007_s11229_020_02655_7
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_infbeh_2019_101350
crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_12891
crossref_primary_10_1371_journal_pone_0241721
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12904
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2018_07_012
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2019_06_009
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyt_2024_1189777
crossref_primary_10_1038_s44159_022_00037_z
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2024_1455941
crossref_primary_10_1002_icd_2348
crossref_primary_10_1111_infa_12277
crossref_primary_10_1002_wcs_1551
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2022_101273
crossref_primary_10_1080_15248372_2018_1544140
crossref_primary_10_1177_0956797617747090
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jecp_2022_105419
crossref_primary_10_1111_infa_12358
crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cogdev_2018_09_005
crossref_primary_10_1080_15475441_2020_1820339
crossref_primary_10_2478_plc_2019_0006
crossref_primary_10_1098_rsos_211278
crossref_primary_10_1111_cdev_13955
crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2023_1142302
Cites_doi 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006
10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105
10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02063.x
10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x
10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.008
10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.618
10.1111/cdev.12311
10.1016/j.rasd.2011.04.004
10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.07.008
10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.08.003
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00946.x
10.1093/scan/nsu081
10.1111/desc.12148
10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00719
10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.001
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00736.x
10.1080/15248372.2011.608198
10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.002
10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199334919.001.0001
10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006
10.1207/s15326942dn2802_5
10.4324/9781315807874
10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00152.x
10.1017/S0305000904006270
10.1016/S0065-2407(09)03702-1
10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.09.001
10.1111/cdep.12183
10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838
10.1111/1467-8624.00333
10.1016/j.psfr.2013.11.002
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x
10.1126/science.1107621
10.1111/mila.12036
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00347.x
10.1177/0963721416673174
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00980.x
10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12
10.1177/0956797611411584
10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4
10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115033
10.1111/desc.12173
10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.014
10.1177/0956797614558717
10.1097/PEP.0b013e31829db85b
10.1017/S0142716400001053
10.1111/1467-8624.00304
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00813.x
10.1016/j.dr.2014.04.001
10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00105.x
10.1080/15248372.2012.689390
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01098.x
10.1126/science.1190792
10.1111/cdev.12533
10.1037/a0016923
10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00478.x
10.1371/journal.pone.0142405
10.1037/14341-003
10.1016/j.cognition.2004.11.005
10.2307/1130386
10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02046.x
10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.06.005
10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4
10.1080/15248372.2015.1086771
10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02060.x
ContentType Journal Article
Copyright 2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Copyright © 2018 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Copyright_xml – notice: 2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
– notice: 2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
– notice: Copyright © 2018 John Wiley & Sons Ltd
DBID AAYXX
CITATION
7SW
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
ERI
PET
REK
WWN
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QP
7TK
7X8
5PM
DOI 10.1111/desc.12600
DatabaseName CrossRef
ERIC
ERIC (Ovid)
ERIC
ERIC
ERIC (Legacy Platform)
ERIC( SilverPlatter )
ERIC
ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform)
Education Resources Information Center (ERIC)
ERIC
Medline
MEDLINE
MEDLINE (Ovid)
MEDLINE
MEDLINE
PubMed
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
PubMed Central (Full Participant titles)
DatabaseTitle CrossRef
ERIC
MEDLINE
Medline Complete
MEDLINE with Full Text
PubMed
MEDLINE (Ovid)
Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
Neurosciences Abstracts
MEDLINE - Academic
DatabaseTitleList MEDLINE
CrossRef


Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts
ERIC
MEDLINE - Academic
Database_xml – sequence: 1
  dbid: NPM
  name: PubMed
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 2
  dbid: EIF
  name: MEDLINE
  url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search
  sourceTypes: Index Database
– sequence: 3
  dbid: ERI
  name: ERIC
  url: https://eric.ed.gov/
  sourceTypes: Index Database
DeliveryMethod fulltext_linktorsrc
Discipline Psychology
EISSN 1467-7687
ERIC EJ1183141
EndPage n/a
ExternalDocumentID PMC5871541
28952180
EJ1183141
10_1111_desc_12600
DESC12600
Genre article
Journal Article
GrantInformation_xml – fundername: Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC)
  funderid: #435‐2012‐1403
– fundername: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD)
  funderid: #R01HD068458
– fundername: NICHD NIH HHS
  grantid: R01 HD068458
GroupedDBID ---
.3N
.GA
.Y3
05W
0R~
10A
1OC
29F
31~
33P
36B
4.4
50Y
50Z
51W
51Y
52M
52O
52Q
52S
52T
52U
52V
52W
53G
5GY
5HH
5LA
5VS
66C
702
7PT
8-0
8-1
8-3
8-4
8-5
8UM
930
A01
A04
AABNI
AAESR
AAHHS
AAHQN
AAHSB
AAIPD
AAMNL
AANHP
AAONW
AAOUF
AASGY
AAXRX
AAYCA
AAZKR
ABCQN
ABCUV
ABDBF
ABEML
ABIVO
ABPVW
ABQWH
ABSOO
ABXGK
ACAHQ
ACBKW
ACBWZ
ACCFJ
ACCZN
ACFBH
ACGFS
ACGOF
ACHQT
ACMXC
ACPOU
ACRPL
ACSCC
ACUHS
ACXQS
ACYXJ
ADBBV
ADBTR
ADEMA
ADEOM
ADIZJ
ADKYN
ADMGS
ADNMO
ADXAS
ADZMN
ADZOD
AEEZP
AEIGN
AEIMD
AEQDE
AEUQT
AEUYR
AFBPY
AFEBI
AFFNX
AFFPM
AFGKR
AFKFF
AFPWT
AFWVQ
AFYRF
AFZJQ
AHBTC
AIACR
AIFKG
AIURR
AIWBW
AJBDE
ALAGY
ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS
ALUQN
ALVPJ
AMBMR
AMYDB
ASPBG
ASTYK
AVWKF
AZBYB
AZFZN
AZVAB
BAFTC
BDRZF
BFHJK
BMXJE
BNVMJ
BQESF
BROTX
BRXPI
BY8
CAG
COF
CS3
D-7
D-C
D-D
DCZOG
DPXWK
DR2
DRFUL
DRMAN
DRSSH
EAD
EAP
EBD
EBS
EJD
EMB
EMK
EMOBN
EPS
EST
ESX
F00
F01
F5P
FEDTE
FUBAC
G-S
G.N
G50
GODZA
HGLYW
HVGLF
HZI
HZ~
IHE
IX1
J0M
K48
KBYEO
LATKE
LC2
LC4
LEEKS
LH4
LITHE
LOXES
LP6
LP7
LUTES
LW6
LYRES
MEWTI
MK4
MRFUL
MRMAN
MRSSH
MSFUL
MSMAN
MSSSH
MXFUL
MXMAN
MXSSH
N04
N06
N9A
NF~
O66
O9-
OIG
OVD
P2P
P2W
P2Y
P4C
PQQKQ
Q.N
Q11
QB0
R.K
ROL
RX1
SUPJJ
SV3
TEORI
TUS
UB1
UPT
UQL
W8V
W99
WBKPD
WHDPE
WIH
WII
WIJ
WOHZO
WQZ
WRC
WSUWO
WXSBR
XG1
ZZTAW
~IA
~WP
AAMMB
AAYXX
AEFGJ
AEYWJ
AGHNM
AGQPQ
AGXDD
AIDQK
AIDYY
CITATION
7SW
BJH
BNH
BNI
BNJ
BNO
ERI
PET
REK
WWN
CGR
CUY
CVF
ECM
EIF
NPM
7QP
7TK
7X8
5PM
ID FETCH-LOGICAL-c4340-a25c05eb27a58637b7f4fb00c39d96835ade190e22a0d034ae6ef63c3d3f66f73
IEDL.DBID DR2
ISSN 1363-755X
1467-7687
IngestDate Thu Aug 21 14:22:05 EDT 2025
Thu Jul 10 16:34:38 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 14 20:43:49 EDT 2025
Mon Jul 21 05:58:19 EDT 2025
Tue Sep 02 20:00:15 EDT 2025
Wed Oct 01 03:30:54 EDT 2025
Thu Apr 24 23:01:25 EDT 2025
Wed Jan 22 16:59:13 EST 2025
IsDoiOpenAccess false
IsOpenAccess false
IsPeerReviewed false
IsScholarly true
Issue 4
Language English
License http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/termsAndConditions#vor
2017 John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
LinkModel DirectLink
MergedId FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c4340-a25c05eb27a58637b7f4fb00c39d96835ade190e22a0d034ae6ef63c3d3f66f73
Notes This research was supported by graduate fellowships from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) to Jessica Yott and research grants from SSHRC (#435‐2012‐1403) and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD; #R01HD068458) to Diane Poulin‐Dubois.
Funding Information
ObjectType-Article-1
SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1
ObjectType-Feature-2
content type line 14
content type line 23
PMID 28952180
PQID 2058553181
PQPubID 2030115
PageCount 11
ParticipantIDs pubmedcentral_primary_oai_pubmedcentral_nih_gov_5871541
proquest_miscellaneous_1943645301
proquest_journals_2058553181
pubmed_primary_28952180
eric_primary_EJ1183141
crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_12600
crossref_citationtrail_10_1111_desc_12600
wiley_primary_10_1111_desc_12600_DESC12600
ProviderPackageCode CITATION
AAYXX
PublicationCentury 2000
PublicationDate July 2018
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD 2018-07-01
PublicationDate_xml – month: 07
  year: 2018
  text: July 2018
PublicationDecade 2010
PublicationPlace England
PublicationPlace_xml – name: England
– name: Oxford
PublicationTitle Developmental science
PublicationTitleAlternate Dev Sci
PublicationYear 2018
Publisher Wiley-Blackwell
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
Publisher_xml – name: Wiley-Blackwell
– name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc
References 1995; 31
2015; 39
2013; 25
2010; 14
2013; 28
2010; 13
2004; 7
2015; 142
2008; 109
2009; 112
2011; 14
2005; 28
2009; 116
2010; 61
2009; 12
2004; 31
2013; 14
2015; 82
2016; 87
2014; 59
2011; 22
2005; 308
2014; 17
2007; 18
2001; 72
2015; 6
2004; 40
2017; 26
2010
2000; 21
1988; 59
2006; 9
2015; 10
2016; 10
1999; 23
2008; 11
2016; 17
2011; 5
2012; 30
1998; 69
1999
2014; 86
1994; 9
2015; 26
2012a; 17
2009; 32
1997; 33
2005; 9
2010; 330
2017
2005; 98
2015
2008; 44
2014
2012b; 15
2012; 6
2014; 34
2009; 37
2016; 67
e_1_2_7_5_1
e_1_2_7_3_1
Trudeau N. (e_1_2_7_64_1) 1999; 23
e_1_2_7_9_1
e_1_2_7_7_1
e_1_2_7_19_1
e_1_2_7_60_1
e_1_2_7_62_1
e_1_2_7_15_1
Mangold P. (e_1_2_7_30_1) 2010
e_1_2_7_41_1
e_1_2_7_13_1
e_1_2_7_43_1
e_1_2_7_66_1
e_1_2_7_11_1
e_1_2_7_45_1
e_1_2_7_68_1
e_1_2_7_47_1
e_1_2_7_26_1
e_1_2_7_49_1
Poulin‐Dubois D. (e_1_2_7_38_1) 1999
Colombo J. (e_1_2_7_17_1) 2014
e_1_2_7_73_1
e_1_2_7_50_1
e_1_2_7_71_1
e_1_2_7_25_1
e_1_2_7_31_1
e_1_2_7_52_1
e_1_2_7_23_1
e_1_2_7_33_1
e_1_2_7_54_1
e_1_2_7_21_1
e_1_2_7_56_1
e_1_2_7_37_1
e_1_2_7_58_1
e_1_2_7_39_1
e_1_2_7_6_1
e_1_2_7_4_1
e_1_2_7_8_1
e_1_2_7_18_1
e_1_2_7_16_1
e_1_2_7_40_1
e_1_2_7_61_1
e_1_2_7_2_1
e_1_2_7_14_1
e_1_2_7_63_1
e_1_2_7_12_1
e_1_2_7_44_1
e_1_2_7_65_1
e_1_2_7_10_1
e_1_2_7_46_1
e_1_2_7_67_1
e_1_2_7_48_1
e_1_2_7_69_1
e_1_2_7_27_1
e_1_2_7_29_1
e_1_2_7_72_1
Powell L. (e_1_2_7_42_1) 2017
e_1_2_7_51_1
e_1_2_7_70_1
e_1_2_7_53_1
e_1_2_7_24_1
e_1_2_7_32_1
e_1_2_7_55_1
e_1_2_7_22_1
Kulke L. (e_1_2_7_28_1) 2017
e_1_2_7_34_1
Perner J. (e_1_2_7_35_1) 2010
e_1_2_7_57_1
e_1_2_7_20_1
e_1_2_7_36_1
e_1_2_7_59_1
References_xml – start-page: 241
  year: 2010
  end-page: 261
– volume: 9
  start-page: 377
  year: 1994
  end-page: 395
  article-title: Implicit understanding of belief
  publication-title: Cognitive Development
– volume: 9
  start-page: 182
  year: 2006
  end-page: 188
  article-title: Change in children's understanding of others’ intentional actions
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 7
  start-page: 283
  year: 2004
  end-page: 288
  article-title: Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 59
  start-page: 59
  year: 2014
  end-page: 69
  article-title: Executive functions and theory of mind understanding in young children: A reciprocal relation?
  publication-title: Psychologie Française
– volume: 30
  start-page: 75
  year: 2012
  end-page: 86
  article-title: Direct and indirect measures of level‐2 perspective‐taking in children and adults
  publication-title: British Journal of Developmental Psychology
– volume: 9
  start-page: 462
  year: 2005
  end-page: 463
  article-title: Do infants really understand false belief?
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
– volume: 59
  start-page: 26
  year: 1988
  end-page: 37
  article-title: Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance–reality distinction
  publication-title: Child Development
– volume: 10
  start-page: 184
  year: 2016
  end-page: 189
  article-title: Cognitive architecture of belief reasoning in children and adults: A primer on the two systems account
  publication-title: Child Development Perspectives
– volume: 17
  start-page: 647
  year: 2014
  end-page: 659
  article-title: False belief in infancy: A fresh look
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 61
  start-page: 366
  year: 2010
  end-page: 395
  article-title: Attributing false beliefs about non‐obvious properties at 18 months
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 25
  start-page: 395
  year: 2013
  end-page: 401
  article-title: Concurrent validity of the TIMP and the Bayley III scales at 6 weeks corrected age
  publication-title: Pediatric Physical Therapy
– year: 2014
– volume: 26
  start-page: 68
  year: 2017
  end-page: 74
  article-title: The developmental origins of false‐belief understanding
  publication-title: Current Directions in Psychological Science
– volume: 308
  start-page: 255
  year: 2005
  end-page: 258
  article-title: Do 15‐month‐old infants understand false beliefs?
  publication-title: Science
– start-page: 193
  year: 2014
  end-page: 229
– volume: 6
  start-page: 719
  year: 2015
  article-title: Developmental pathways for social understanding: Linking social cognition to social contexts
  publication-title: Frontiers in Psychology
– volume: 15
  start-page: 113
  year: 2012b
  end-page: 122
  article-title: Eighteen‐ and 24‐month‐old infants correct others in anticipation of action mistakes
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 11
  start-page: 862
  year: 2008
  end-page: 868
  article-title: The link between infant attention to goal‐directed action and later theory of mind abilities
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– start-page: 257
  year: 1999
  end-page: 280
– volume: 109
  start-page: 295
  year: 2008
  end-page: 315
  article-title: Can an agent's false belief be corrected by an appropriate communication? Psychological reasoning in 18‐month‐old infants
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 69
  start-page: 1
  issue: 1
  year: 1998
  end-page: 34
  article-title: Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach
  publication-title: Cognition
– start-page: 79
  year: 2015
  end-page: 150
– volume: 30
  start-page: 30
  year: 2012
  end-page: 44
  article-title: Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to a geometric shape at 17 months
  publication-title: British Journal of Developmental Psychology
– volume: 17
  start-page: 660
  year: 2014
  end-page: 664
  article-title: How fresh a look? A reply to Heyes
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 14
  start-page: 110
  year: 2010
  end-page: 118
  article-title: False‐belief understanding in infants
  publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences
– volume: 28
  start-page: 645
  year: 2005
  end-page: 668
  article-title: Executive function and theory of mind in 2 year olds: A family affair?
  publication-title: Developmental Neuropsychology
– volume: 23
  start-page: 61
  year: 1999
  end-page: 73
  article-title: Une adaptation en français québécois du MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory [A Quebec‐French adaptation of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory]
  publication-title: La revue d'orthophonie et d'audiologie
– volume: 330
  start-page: 1830
  year: 2010
  end-page: 1834
  article-title: The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults
  publication-title: Science
– volume: 28
  start-page: 606
  year: 2013
  end-page: 637
  article-title: How to construct a minimal theory of mind
  publication-title: Mind & Language
– volume: 14
  start-page: 292
  year: 2011
  end-page: 305
  article-title: False‐belief understanding in 2.5‐year‐olds: Evidence from violation of expectation change of location and unexpected‐contents tasks
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 39
  start-page: 63
  year: 2015
  end-page: 72
  article-title: How to help: Can more active behavioral measures help transcend the infant false belief debate?
  publication-title: New Ideas in Psychology
– volume: 18
  start-page: 587
  year: 2007
  end-page: 592
  article-title: Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2‐year‐olds
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 98
  start-page: 137
  year: 2005
  end-page: 155
  article-title: Infants’ understanding of object‐directed action
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 26
  start-page: 1353
  year: 2015
  end-page: 1367
  article-title: A second look at automatic theory of mind: Reconsidering Kovács, Téglás, and Endress (2010)
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 14
  start-page: 87
  year: 2013
  end-page: 99
  article-title: Is false belief skin‐deep? The agent's eye status influences infants’ reasoning in belief‐inducing situations
  publication-title: Journal of Cognition and Development
– volume: 5
  start-page: 39
  year: 2011
  end-page: 43
  article-title: Theory of mind in infancy
  publication-title: Child Development Perspectives
– volume: 31
  start-page: 587
  year: 2004
  end-page: 608
  article-title: Measuring productive vocabulary of toddlers in low‐income families: Concurrent and predictive validity of three sources of data
  publication-title: Journal of Child Language
– volume: 32
  start-page: 404
  year: 2009
  end-page: 415
  article-title: Infants’ understanding of intention from 10 to 14 months: Interrelations among violation of expectancy and imitation tasks
  publication-title: Infant Behavior and Development
– volume: 33
  start-page: 12
  year: 1997
  end-page: 21
  article-title: Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14‐ and 18‐month‐olds
  publication-title: Developmental Psychology
– volume: 86
  start-page: 486
  year: 2014
  end-page: 502
  article-title: Explicit theory of mind is even more unified than previously assumed: Belief ascription and understanding aspectuality emerge together in development
  publication-title: Child Development
– volume: 87
  start-page: 1221
  year: 2016
  end-page: 1232
  article-title: Understanding of goals, beliefs, and desires predicts morally relevant theory of mind: A longitudinal investigation
  publication-title: Child Development
– volume: 14
  start-page: 480
  year: 2013
  end-page: 498
  article-title: Concurrent relations between perspective‐taking skills, desire understanding, and internal‐state vocabulary
  publication-title: Journal of Cognition and Development
– volume: 72
  start-page: 1032
  year: 2001
  end-page: 1053
  article-title: Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind
  publication-title: Child Development
– volume: 40
  start-page: 1105
  year: 2004
  end-page: 1122
  article-title: Executive function and theory of mind: Stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3
  publication-title: Developmental Psychology
– volume: 17
  start-page: 672
  year: 2012a
  end-page: 691
  article-title: 18‐month‐olds predict specific action mistakes through attribution of false belief, not ignorance, and intervene accordingly
  publication-title: Infancy
– volume: 12
  start-page: 746
  year: 2009
  end-page: 752
  article-title: Continuity in social cognition from infancy to childhood
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 6
  start-page: 184
  year: 2012
  end-page: 192
  article-title: Modified checklist for autism in toddlers (M‐CHAT) screening at 18 months of age predicts concurrent understanding of desires, word learning and expressive vocabulary
  publication-title: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
– volume: 30
  start-page: 156
  year: 2012
  end-page: 171
  article-title: Breaking the rules: Do infants have a true understanding of false belief
  publication-title: British Journal of Developmental Psychology
– year: 2010
– volume: 10
  start-page: e0142405
  year: 2015
  article-title: Processing demands impact 3‐year‐olds’ performance in a spontaneous‐response task: New evidence for the processing‐load account of early false‐belief understanding
  publication-title: PLoS ONE
– volume: 22
  start-page: 878
  year: 2011
  end-page: 880
  article-title: Do 18‐month‐olds really attribute mental states to others? A critical test
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 10
  start-page: 327
  year: 2015
  end-page: 334
  article-title: Spontaneous mentalizing captures variability in the cortical thickness of social brain regions
  publication-title: Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience
– volume: 44
  start-page: 618
  year: 2008
  end-page: 623
  article-title: Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind
  publication-title: Developmental Psychology
– volume: 31
  start-page: 838
  year: 1995
  end-page: 850
  article-title: Understanding the intentions of others: Re‐enactment of intended acts by 18‐month‐old children
  publication-title: Developmental Psychology
– volume: 112
  start-page: 337
  year: 2009
  end-page: 342
  article-title: Eighteen‐month‐old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 67
  start-page: 159
  year: 2016
  end-page: 186
  article-title: Psychological reasoning in infancy
  publication-title: Annual Review of Psychology
– volume: 13
  start-page: 907
  year: 2010
  end-page: 912
  article-title: Seventeen‐month‐olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication
  publication-title: Developmental Science
– volume: 30
  start-page: 172
  year: 2012
  end-page: 187
  article-title: Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age
  publication-title: British Journal of Developmental Psychology
– volume: 82
  start-page: 32
  year: 2015
  end-page: 56
  article-title: Infants understand deceptive intentions to implant false beliefs about identity: New evidence for early mentalistic reasoning
  publication-title: Cognitive Psychology
– volume: 17
  start-page: 683
  year: 2016
  end-page: 698
  article-title: Are infants’ theory of mind abilities well integrated? Implicit understanding of intentions, desires, and beliefs
  publication-title: Journal of Cognition and Development
– year: 2017
– volume: 21
  start-page: 95
  year: 2000
  end-page: 115
  article-title: Short‐form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories
  publication-title: Applied Psycholinguistics
– volume: 37
  start-page: 55
  year: 2009
  end-page: 104
  article-title: The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy
  publication-title: Advances in Child Development and Behavior
– volume: 34
  start-page: 265
  year: 2014
  end-page: 293
  article-title: To belief or not belief: Children's theory of mind
  publication-title: Developmental Review
– volume: 142
  start-page: 44
  year: 2015
  end-page: 59
  article-title: Goal attributions and instrumental helping at 14 and 24 months of age
  publication-title: Cognition
– volume: 18
  start-page: 580
  year: 2007
  end-page: 586
  article-title: Attribution of beliefs by 13‐month‐old infants
  publication-title: Psychological Science
– volume: 116
  start-page: 953
  year: 2009
  end-page: 970
  article-title: Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief‐like states?
  publication-title: Psychological Review
– volume: 72
  start-page: 655
  year: 2001
  end-page: 684
  article-title: Meta‐analysis of theory‐of‐mind development: The truth about false belief
  publication-title: Child Development
– ident: e_1_2_7_6_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006
– ident: e_1_2_7_13_1
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1105
– ident: e_1_2_7_62_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02063.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_63_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_57_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.008
– ident: e_1_2_7_67_1
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.618
– ident: e_1_2_7_44_1
  doi: 10.1111/cdev.12311
– ident: e_1_2_7_70_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.04.004
– ident: e_1_2_7_2_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2015.07.008
– ident: e_1_2_7_52_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2015.08.003
– ident: e_1_2_7_58_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00946.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_46_1
  doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu081
– ident: e_1_2_7_22_1
  doi: 10.1111/desc.12148
– ident: e_1_2_7_9_1
  doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00719
– ident: e_1_2_7_48_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.001
– ident: e_1_2_7_4_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00736.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_40_1
  doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.608198
– ident: e_1_2_7_43_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.002
– ident: e_1_2_7_65_1
  doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199334919.001.0001
– start-page: 257
  volume-title: Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life
  year: 1999
  ident: e_1_2_7_38_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_10_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006
– ident: e_1_2_7_24_1
  doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_5
– volume: 23
  start-page: 61
  year: 1999
  ident: e_1_2_7_64_1
  article-title: Une adaptation en français québécois du MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory [A Quebec‐French adaptation of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory]
  publication-title: La revue d'orthophonie et d'audiologie
– start-page: 193
  volume-title: Individual differences in infancy: Reliability, stability, and prediction
  year: 2014
  ident: e_1_2_7_17_1
  doi: 10.4324/9781315807874
– ident: e_1_2_7_55_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00152.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_34_1
  doi: 10.1017/S0305000904006270
– ident: e_1_2_7_39_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0065-2407(09)03702-1
– ident: e_1_2_7_51_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.09.001
– ident: e_1_2_7_29_1
  doi: 10.1111/cdep.12183
– ident: e_1_2_7_31_1
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838
– start-page: 241
  volume-title: Cognition and neuropsychology: International perspectives on psychological science
  year: 2010
  ident: e_1_2_7_35_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_14_1
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00333
– volume-title: Interact user guide, V. 8.0
  year: 2010
  ident: e_1_2_7_30_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_41_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.psfr.2013.11.002
– ident: e_1_2_7_59_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x
– volume-title: Are implicit theory of mind findings robust? Some doubts from converging non‐replications across the lifespan
  year: 2017
  ident: e_1_2_7_42_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_60_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_33_1
  doi: 10.1126/science.1107621
– ident: e_1_2_7_11_1
  doi: 10.1111/mila.12036
– ident: e_1_2_7_68_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00347.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_49_1
  doi: 10.1177/0963721416673174
– ident: e_1_2_7_21_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00980.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_45_1
  doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12
– ident: e_1_2_7_54_1
  doi: 10.1177/0956797611411584
– ident: e_1_2_7_69_1
  doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4
– ident: e_1_2_7_5_1
  doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115033
– ident: e_1_2_7_50_1
  doi: 10.1111/desc.12173
– ident: e_1_2_7_23_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.03.014
– ident: e_1_2_7_37_1
  doi: 10.1177/0956797614558717
– ident: e_1_2_7_12_1
  doi: 10.1097/PEP.0b013e31829db85b
– ident: e_1_2_7_19_1
  doi: 10.1017/S0142716400001053
– ident: e_1_2_7_66_1
  doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00304
– ident: e_1_2_7_71_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00813.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_47_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.04.001
– ident: e_1_2_7_25_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00105.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_15_1
  doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.689390
– ident: e_1_2_7_26_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01098.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_27_1
  doi: 10.1126/science.1190792
– volume-title: Are implicit theory of mind findings robust? Some doubts from converging non‐replications across the lifespan
  year: 2017
  ident: e_1_2_7_28_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_56_1
  doi: 10.1111/cdev.12533
– ident: e_1_2_7_3_1
  doi: 10.1037/a0016923
– ident: e_1_2_7_8_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00478.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_53_1
  doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142405
– ident: e_1_2_7_7_1
  doi: 10.1037/14341-003
– ident: e_1_2_7_36_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.11.005
– ident: e_1_2_7_20_1
  doi: 10.2307/1130386
– ident: e_1_2_7_61_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02046.x
– ident: e_1_2_7_32_1
  doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.06.005
– ident: e_1_2_7_18_1
– ident: e_1_2_7_16_1
  doi: 10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4
– ident: e_1_2_7_73_1
  doi: 10.1080/15248372.2015.1086771
– ident: e_1_2_7_72_1
  doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02060.x
SSID ssj0004301
Score 2.4556956
Snippet There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true theory of mind (ToM) understanding. According to the mentalistic...
There is currently a hot debate in the literature regarding whether or not infants have a true ToM understanding. According to the mentalistic view, infants...
SourceID pubmedcentral
proquest
pubmed
eric
crossref
wiley
SourceType Open Access Repository
Aggregation Database
Index Database
Enrichment Source
Publisher
StartPage e12600
SubjectTerms Aptitude
Attention
Beliefs
Child
Children
Cognition
Cognitive Ability
Comparative Analysis
Comprehension
Correlation
Female
Humans
Infant
Infants
Intention
Interaction
Male
Motivation
Psychological Patterns
Reproducibility of Results
Social Perception
Task Analysis
Task Performance and Analysis
Theory of Mind
Theory of Mind - physiology
Validity
Title Probing the depth of infants’ theory of mind: disunity in performance across paradigms
URI https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2Fdesc.12600
http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1183141
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28952180
https://www.proquest.com/docview/2058553181
https://www.proquest.com/docview/1943645301
https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/PMC5871541
Volume 21
hasFullText 1
inHoldings 1
isFullTextHit
isPrint
journalDatabaseRights – providerCode: PRVEBS
  databaseName: Academic Search Ultimate
  customDbUrl: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?authtype=ip,shib&custid=s3936755&profile=ehost&defaultdb=asn
  eissn: 1467-7687
  dateEnd: 20241001
  omitProxy: true
  ssIdentifier: ssj0004301
  issn: 1363-755X
  databaseCode: ABDBF
  dateStart: 19980401
  isFulltext: true
  titleUrlDefault: https://search.ebscohost.com/direct.asp?db=asn
  providerName: EBSCOhost
– providerCode: PRVWIB
  databaseName: Wiley Online Library - Core collection (SURFmarket)
  issn: 1363-755X
  databaseCode: DR2
  dateStart: 19980101
  customDbUrl:
  isFulltext: true
  eissn: 1467-7687
  dateEnd: 99991231
  omitProxy: false
  ssIdentifier: ssj0004301
  providerName: Wiley-Blackwell
link http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9QwELaqnnoBSikstJURXEDKyo6fqbigdquqEggBlfaCIsd22hU0u2p2D8uJv8Hf45cwdh67CwgJblE8ediemXxjz3xB6Hnwi1R7lUCwnCbcO5sUlmvQ5VQ67TxhRShwfvNWnl_yi7EYb6FXXS1Mww_RL7gFy4j-Ohi4Keo1I3e-tkMa-NXBAVMm4h7t-xV3FGekLbpiiRJi3HKThjSe1aUbX6PNhOc1pPl7wuQ6kI1forO76FPXhyYB5fNwMS-G9usv9I7_28l76E4LUfHrRqd20Zav7qOd3lMu99D4XWBvqq4woEfs_Gx-jaclBl0NSTU_vn3HsTxyGU7eQMx_jN2kXoDvWIIMnq1KFbCJw4ID_7ibXN3UD9Dl2ejjyXnS_qIhsZxxkphUWCIgOldGaMlUoUpegiVblrlMArozzgPk8GlqiCOMGy99KZlljpVSlorto-1qWvlHCGvLDaGGqIxYTqky0pYa7pmWzmeWsQF60U1Vblv-8vAbjS95F8eEwcrjYA3Qs1521rB2_FFqP8x4LzG6gGiLUU4H6KDTgbw16TpPCURW4LE0ND_tm8EYww6Lqfx0Uec042FbFzRvgB42KtPfHSJbgEoanqo2lKkXCETfmy3V5DoSfguIakV4rZdRV_7Spfx09OEkHj3-F-EnaAeAoG7SkA_Q9vx24Q8BbM2Lo2hUR3FZ4Se_Iybz
linkProvider Wiley-Blackwell
linkToHtml http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1fb9MwELdgPLAX_g8KA4zgBaRUTuzYDm9o61TGNiHYpL5Fju1sFSytSPtQnvgafD0-CXdOmraAkOAtii9OYt9dfhff_UzIC_SLsfYqgmA5iYR3Niqs0KDLiXTaecYLLHA-PpHDM3E4Skdtbg7WwjT8EN0PN7SM4K_RwPGH9JqVO1_bfowE61fJNVygQ7vc_7BijxKctWVXPFJpOmrZSTGRZ3XtxvdoM-V5DWv-njK5DmXDt-jgZrPhah0oDDEF5VN_Piv69usvBI___Zq3yI0WpdI3jVrdJld8dYdsd85ycZeM3iOBU3VOAUBS56ezCzopKagr5tX8-PadhgrJBZ68hLD_NXXjeg7uYwEydLqqVqAmjAtFCnI3Pr-s75Gzg8Hp3jBqd2mIrOCCRSZJLUshQFcm1ZKrQpWiBGO2PHOZBIBnnAfU4ZPEMMe4MF76UnLLHS-lLBXfIVvVpPIPCNVWGBYbpjJmRRwrI22poc-kdD6znPfIy-Vc5balMMedND7ny1AGBysPg9UjzzvZaUPc8UepHZzyTmJwCAEXj0XcI7tLJchbq67zhEFwBU5LQ_OzrhnsERdZTOUn8zqPM4Eru6B6PXK_0ZmudwhuAS1puKva0KZOALm-N1uq8UXg_E4hsE3xsV4FZfnLK-X7g4974ejhvwg_JdeHp8dH-dHbk3ePyDbgQt1kJe-SrdmXuX8M2GtWPAkW9hMYaCoO
linkToPdf http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1Lb9NAEB6VIqFeKK-WlAKL4AKSo7V3vV4jLqhNVApUFVApF2St99FGUCfCySGc-Bv8PX4Js34lAYQEtyg7cbzrmfH3eWc-AzzxeTGUNgmQLEcBt0YHueYSfTkSRhpLWe4bnN-eiKMzfjyKRxvwou2FqfUhugduPjKqfO0DfGrcSpAbW-p-6PXVr8BVLpBeeUj0bikexRltuq5YkMTxqBEn9XU8y9-u3Y7WK55XoObvFZOrSLa6FQ234WM7iboC5VN_Psv7-usv-o7_O8sbcL3BqORl7VQ3YcMWt2CrS5WL2zA69fJNxTlB-EiMnc4uyMQRdFZfVfPj23dS9Ucu_JeXSPqfEzMu55g8FmhDpsteBaKqZSFegNyMzy_LO3A2HHw4OAqadzQEmjNOAxXFmsZIzxMVS8GSPHHcYShrlppUILxTxiLmsFGkqKGMKyusE0wzw5wQLmE7sFlMCnsXiNRc0VDRJKWah2GihHYSjxk5Y1PNWA-etpcq042AuX-PxuesJTJ-sbJqsXrwuLOd1rIdf7Ta8Ve8sxgcI91iIQ97sN_6QNbEdJlFFKkVpiyJw4-6YYxGv8WiCjuZl1mYcr-vi57Xg93aZbqjI7VFrCTxX5M1Z-oMvNL3-kgxvqgUv2OktbE_rWeVr_xlStnh4P1B9WnvX4wfwrXTw2H25tXJ63uwhaBQ1iXJ-7A5-zK39xF4zfIHVXz9BIHnKL0
openUrl ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Probing+the+depth+of+infants%27+theory+of+mind%3A+disunity+in+performance+across+paradigms&rft.jtitle=Developmental+science&rft.au=Poulin-Dubois%2C+Diane&rft.au=Yott%2C+Jessica&rft.date=2018-07-01&rft.eissn=1467-7687&rft.volume=21&rft.issue=4&rft.spage=e12600&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2Fdesc.12600&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F28952180&rft.externalDocID=28952180
thumbnail_l http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon
thumbnail_m http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon
thumbnail_s http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon