Representing agents, patients, goals and instruments in causative events: A cross‐linguistic investigation of early language and cognition
Although it is widely assumed that the linguistic description of events is based on a structured representation of event components at the perceptual/conceptual level, little empirical work has tested this assumption directly. Here, we test the connection between language and perception/cognition cr...
Saved in:
Published in | Developmental science Vol. 24; no. 6; pp. e13116 - n/a |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
England
Wiley
01.11.2021
Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1363-755X 1467-7687 1467-7687 |
DOI | 10.1111/desc.13116 |
Cover
Abstract | Although it is widely assumed that the linguistic description of events is based on a structured representation of event components at the perceptual/conceptual level, little empirical work has tested this assumption directly. Here, we test the connection between language and perception/cognition cross‐linguistically, focusing on the relative salience of causative event components in language and cognition. We draw on evidence from preschoolers speaking English or Turkish. In a picture description task, Turkish‐speaking 3‐5‐year‐olds mentioned Agents less than their English‐speaking peers (Turkish allows subject drop); furthermore, both language groups mentioned Patients more frequently than Goals, and Instruments less frequently than either Patients or Goals. In a change blindness task, both language groups were equally accurate at detecting changes to Agents (despite surface differences in Agent mentions). The remaining components also behaved similarly: both language groups were less accurate in detecting changes to Instruments than either Patients or Goals (even though Turkish‐speaking preschoolers were less accurate overall than their English‐speaking peers). To our knowledge, this is the first study offering evidence for a strong—even though not strict—homology between linguistic and conceptual event roles in young learners cross‐linguistically. |
---|---|
AbstractList | Although it is widely assumed that the linguistic description of events is based on a structured representation of event components at the perceptual/conceptual level, little empirical work has tested this assumption directly. Here, we test the connection between language and perception/cognition cross‐linguistically, focusing on the relative salience of causative event components in language and cognition. We draw on evidence from preschoolers speaking English or Turkish. In a picture description task, Turkish‐speaking 3‐5‐year‐olds mentioned Agents less than their English‐speaking peers (Turkish allows subject drop); furthermore, both language groups mentioned Patients more frequently than Goals, and Instruments less frequently than either Patients or Goals. In a change blindness task, both language groups were equally accurate at detecting changes to Agents (despite surface differences in Agent mentions). The remaining components also behaved similarly: both language groups were less accurate in detecting changes to Instruments than either Patients or Goals (even though Turkish‐speaking preschoolers were less accurate overall than their English‐speaking peers). To our knowledge, this is the first study offering evidence for a strong—even though not strict—homology between linguistic and conceptual event roles in young learners cross‐linguistically. Although it is widely assumed that the linguistic description of events is based on a structured representation of event components at the perceptual/conceptual level, little empirical work has tested this assumption directly. Here, we test the connection between language and perception/cognition cross-linguistically, focusing on the relative salience of causative event components in language and cognition. We draw on evidence from preschoolers speaking English or Turkish. In a picture description task, Turkish-speaking 3-5-year-olds mentioned Agents less than their English-speaking peers (Turkish allows subject drop); furthermore, both language groups mentioned Patients more frequently than Goals, and Instruments less frequently than either Patients or Goals. In a change blindness task, both language groups were equally accurate at detecting changes to Agents (despite surface differences in Agent mentions). The remaining components also behaved similarly: both language groups were less accurate in detecting changes to Instruments than either Patients or Goals (even though Turkish-speaking preschoolers were less accurate overall than their English-speaking peers). To our knowledge, this is the first study offering evidence for a strong-even though not strict-homology between linguistic and conceptual event roles in young learners cross-linguistically.Although it is widely assumed that the linguistic description of events is based on a structured representation of event components at the perceptual/conceptual level, little empirical work has tested this assumption directly. Here, we test the connection between language and perception/cognition cross-linguistically, focusing on the relative salience of causative event components in language and cognition. We draw on evidence from preschoolers speaking English or Turkish. In a picture description task, Turkish-speaking 3-5-year-olds mentioned Agents less than their English-speaking peers (Turkish allows subject drop); furthermore, both language groups mentioned Patients more frequently than Goals, and Instruments less frequently than either Patients or Goals. In a change blindness task, both language groups were equally accurate at detecting changes to Agents (despite surface differences in Agent mentions). The remaining components also behaved similarly: both language groups were less accurate in detecting changes to Instruments than either Patients or Goals (even though Turkish-speaking preschoolers were less accurate overall than their English-speaking peers). To our knowledge, this is the first study offering evidence for a strong-even though not strict-homology between linguistic and conceptual event roles in young learners cross-linguistically. |
Author | Ünal, Ercenur Trueswell, John C. Papafragou, Anna Richards, Catherine |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Ercenur orcidid: 0000-0002-6794-2129 surname: Ünal fullname: Ünal, Ercenur email: ercenur.unal@ozyegin.edu.tr organization: University of Delaware – sequence: 2 givenname: Catherine surname: Richards fullname: Richards, Catherine organization: University of Delaware – sequence: 3 givenname: John C. surname: Trueswell fullname: Trueswell, John C. organization: University of Pennsylvania – sequence: 4 givenname: Anna surname: Papafragou fullname: Papafragou, Anna organization: University of Pennsylvania |
BackLink | http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1314959$$DView record in ERIC https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33955664$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNp9kcuO1DAQRS00iHnAhj3IEhuEyGDHiR2zGzXNSyMh8ZDYWY5TiTxKO42dNOodH8CCb-RLqHRmWIwQ3viq6tStUtUpOQpDAEIecnbO8b1oILlzLjiXd8gJL6TKlKzUEWohRabK8usxOU3pijFWCMbvkWMhdFlKWZyQnx9hGyFBGH3oqO1QpOd0a0e_qG6wfaI2NNSHNMZpM4dRU2enhNQOKOzm2Et6QV0cUvr941ePXpNPo3dI7gBFh-gQ6NBSsLHf094igd0Ozm7ogp_z98ndFtvBg-v_jHx5vf68eptdfnjzbnVxmTmhtMwKa3nOuQYHqtbK2bKpmoZXjGsutICW6Yq3oJnWImdFLYQSzEmmFK8bB7U4I08X320cvk04n9n45KDHqWCYksnLPJc5kwVH9Mkt9GqYYsDpkKpEUWjJSqQeX1NTvYHGbKPf2Lg3N3tG4NECQPTub3r9Hq9W6FJjni35wwojtMb58bCzMVrfG87MfGozn9ocTo0lz26V3Nj-E-YL_N33sP8PaV6tP62Wmj_r6bqG |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1515_cog_2023_0127 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jml_2023_104475 crossref_primary_10_1111_cogs_13140 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2022_105248 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_actpsy_2024_104639 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_isci_2024_109996 crossref_primary_10_1177_09567976221140328 crossref_primary_10_3389_fpsyg_2021_684022 crossref_primary_10_1126_sciadv_abn8464 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2023_105505 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_cognition_2024_105868 crossref_primary_10_1177_09637214241242460 crossref_primary_10_1162_opmi_a_00083 crossref_primary_10_1017_langcog_2023_61 crossref_primary_10_1017_langcog_2023_72 crossref_primary_10_1017_langcog_2024_40 |
Cites_doi | 10.1080/23273798.2019.1589540 10.1038/nature02634 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00009-2 10.1007/978-94-011-5420-8_2 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104447 10.1037/a0030045 10.1080/15475441.2014.896168 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00128-1 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.07.004 10.3758/BF03196312 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00505-3 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.3 10.1111/1467-9280.00255 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4 10.1080/23273798.2019.1584323 10.1111/1467-8624.00310 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90030-8 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00046-X 10.3758/s13423-019-01634-5 10.1207/s15327817la0101_2 10.1177/1745691609356783 10.1093/obo/9780199772810-0141 10.1111/tops.12475 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90033-Z 10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.82 10.1353/lan.1991.0021 10.1080/23273798.2016.1175649 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6294-11.2012 10.1037/0012-1649.29.3.421 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.006 10.1515/9783110226614.399 10.1007/978-3-319-24277-4 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90015-6 10.1002/0470018860.s00567 10.1016/S0010-0277(87)80006-9 10.1017/CBO9780511613609 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.026 10.1080/01690965.2013.824993 10.1037/dev0000693 10.1111/cogs.12790 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01649.x 10.1111/cogs.12332 10.1080/01690960903017000 10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.007 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.015 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00661-H 10.7551/mitpress/6393.001.0001 10.1515/9783110217117.15 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 10.1016/0010-0277(89)90001-2 10.1111/lang.12188 10.1080/15475440701360168 10.1093/cercor/bht275 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01107.x 10.1017/CBO9780511610479 10.1080/15326900701399954 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.04.003 10.1080/15475441.2018.1544075 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.002 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.03.009 10.4324/9780203340769 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01220.x 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.05.005 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.011 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01117.x |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | 2021 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. |
Copyright_xml | – notice: 2021 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO ERI PET REK WWN CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QP 7TK 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1111/desc.13116 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef ERIC ERIC (Ovid) ERIC ERIC ERIC (Legacy Platform) ERIC( SilverPlatter ) ERIC ERIC PlusText (Legacy Platform) Education Resources Information Center (ERIC) ERIC Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef ERIC MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts Neurosciences Abstracts MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | Calcium & Calcified Tissue Abstracts MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef ERIC |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 3 dbid: ERI name: ERIC url: https://eric.ed.gov/ sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Psychology |
EISSN | 1467-7687 |
ERIC | EJ1314959 |
EndPage | n/a |
ExternalDocumentID | 33955664 EJ1314959 10_1111_desc_13116 DESC13116 |
Genre | article Journal Article Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural |
GrantInformation_xml | – fundername: Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development funderid: R01HD055498 – fundername: NICHD NIH HHS grantid: R01 HD055498 |
GroupedDBID | --- .3N .GA .Y3 05W 0R~ 10A 1OC 29F 31~ 33P 36B 4.4 50Y 50Z 51W 51Y 52M 52O 52Q 52S 52T 52U 52V 52W 53G 5GY 5HH 5LA 5VS 66C 702 7PT 8-0 8-1 8-3 8-4 8-5 8UM 930 A01 A04 AABNI AAESR AAHHS AAHQN AAHSB AAIPD AAMNL AANHP AAONW AAOUF AASGY AAXRX AAYCA AAZKR ABCQN ABCUV ABDBF ABEML ABIVO ABPVW ABQWH ABSOO ABXGK ACAHQ ACBKW ACBWZ ACCFJ ACCZN ACFBH ACGFS ACGOF ACHQT ACMXC ACPOU ACRPL ACSCC ACUHS ACXQS ACYXJ ADBBV ADBTR ADEMA ADEOM ADIZJ ADKYN ADMGS ADNMO ADXAS ADZMN ADZOD AEEZP AEIGN AEIMD AEQDE AEUQT AEUYR AFBPY AFEBI AFFNX AFFPM AFGKR AFKFF AFPWT AFWVQ AFYRF AFZJQ AHBTC AIACR AIFKG AIURR AIWBW AJBDE ALAGY ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALUQN ALVPJ AMBMR AMYDB ASPBG ASTYK AVWKF AZBYB AZFZN AZVAB BAFTC BDRZF BFHJK BMXJE BNVMJ BQESF BROTX BRXPI BY8 CAG COF CS3 D-7 D-C D-D DCZOG DPXWK DR2 DRFUL DRMAN DRSSH EAD EAP EBD EBS EJD EMB EMK EMOBN EPS EST ESX F00 F01 F5P FEDTE FUBAC G-S G.N G50 GODZA HGLYW HVGLF HZI HZ~ IHE IX1 J0M K48 KBYEO LATKE LC2 LC4 LEEKS LH4 LITHE LOXES LP6 LP7 LUTES LW6 LYRES MEWTI MK4 MRFUL MRMAN MRSSH MSFUL MSMAN MSSSH MXFUL MXMAN MXSSH N04 N06 N9A NF~ O66 O9- OIG OVD P2P P2W P2Y P4C PQQKQ Q.N Q11 QB0 R.K ROL RX1 SUPJJ SV3 TEORI TUS UB1 UPT UQL W8V W99 WBKPD WHDPE WIH WII WIJ WOHZO WQZ WRC WSUWO WXSBR XG1 ZZTAW ~IA ~WP AAMMB AAYXX AEFGJ AEYWJ AGHNM AGQPQ AGXDD AIDQK AIDYY CITATION 7SW BJH BNH BNI BNJ BNO ERI PET REK WWN CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7QP 7TK 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c3796-4aa12119ece7b97ca5d8dd180191393ef0981fe90993204b33730c60771bdceb3 |
IEDL.DBID | DR2 |
ISSN | 1363-755X 1467-7687 |
IngestDate | Thu Jul 10 17:24:14 EDT 2025 Fri Jul 25 06:46:34 EDT 2025 Wed Feb 19 02:27:09 EST 2025 Tue Sep 02 18:30:05 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 22:55:55 EDT 2025 Wed Oct 01 03:30:56 EDT 2025 Wed Jan 22 16:27:19 EST 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | false |
IsPeerReviewed | false |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 6 |
Keywords | change blindness cross-linguistic differences event cognition causative events thematic roles event perception |
Language | English |
License | 2021 John Wiley & Sons Ltd. |
LinkModel | DirectLink |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c3796-4aa12119ece7b97ca5d8dd180191393ef0981fe90993204b33730c60771bdceb3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 14 content type line 23 |
ORCID | 0000-0002-6794-2129 |
PMID | 33955664 |
PQID | 2583449605 |
PQPubID | 2030115 |
PageCount | 13 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_2522620641 proquest_journals_2583449605 pubmed_primary_33955664 eric_primary_EJ1314959 crossref_citationtrail_10_1111_desc_13116 crossref_primary_10_1111_desc_13116 wiley_primary_10_1111_desc_13116_DESC13116 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | November 2021 2021-11-00 20211101 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2021-11-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 11 year: 2021 text: November 2021 |
PublicationDecade | 2020 |
PublicationPlace | England |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: England – name: Oxford |
PublicationTitle | Developmental science |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Dev Sci |
PublicationYear | 2021 |
Publisher | Wiley Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
Publisher_xml | – name: Wiley – name: Wiley Subscription Services, Inc |
References | 2007; 102 2017; 41 2012; 122 1993; 29 2015; 142 2019; 15 2016; 31 2020; 205 2014; 29 2007; 31 1997; 8 2018; 175 2007; 136 2010; 25 1989; 32 1990 2019b; 43 2002; 84 1991; 41 2003; 7 2000; 11 2019; 26 1995; 21 2003; 48 1984 1983 1981 2017; 164 1978; 24 2007; 3 2010; 5 2019a; 55 1989 2007; 125 2010; 34 2001; 72 2011; 1 2002; 9 2011 2010 2006; 98 1995; 56 2019; 34 2015; 11 2009 1997 1996 2006 2005 2004 2003 2013; 142 2012; 36 1987; 19 2019; 183 2007; 57 2012; 32 1998; 66 1956 1998; 69 2001; 127 2015; 67 2004; 430 1987; 25 2021; 13 1990; 1 2015; 25 1991; 67 2019 2005; 96 2018 2016 2002b 2014 2002a 2009; 4 2005; 16 1968 2016; 66 e_1_2_8_28_1 e_1_2_8_24_1 e_1_2_8_47_1 e_1_2_8_49_1 e_1_2_8_68_1 e_1_2_8_3_1 e_1_2_8_5_1 Erguvanlı E. E. (e_1_2_8_17_1) 1984 e_1_2_8_7_1 e_1_2_8_9_1 e_1_2_8_20_1 e_1_2_8_43_1 e_1_2_8_22_1 e_1_2_8_64_1 e_1_2_8_87_1 Schneider W. (e_1_2_8_73_1) 2002 e_1_2_8_62_1 e_1_2_8_85_1 e_1_2_8_60_1 e_1_2_8_19_1 Kornfilt J. (e_1_2_8_45_1) 1997 e_1_2_8_13_1 e_1_2_8_36_1 Levelt W. (e_1_2_8_53_1) 1989 e_1_2_8_59_1 e_1_2_8_15_1 e_1_2_8_38_1 e_1_2_8_57_1 Jackendoff R. (e_1_2_8_40_1) 1990 Grimshaw J. (e_1_2_8_33_1) 1981 Bock K. (e_1_2_8_6_1) 2004 e_1_2_8_70_1 e_1_2_8_32_1 e_1_2_8_55_1 e_1_2_8_78_1 e_1_2_8_11_1 e_1_2_8_76_1 Wilson F. (e_1_2_8_83_1) 2011 e_1_2_8_30_1 e_1_2_8_72_1 e_1_2_8_29_1 e_1_2_8_25_1 e_1_2_8_46_1 Jackendoff R. (e_1_2_8_39_1) 1983 e_1_2_8_48_1 e_1_2_8_69_1 e_1_2_8_2_1 e_1_2_8_80_1 e_1_2_8_4_1 Pinker S. (e_1_2_8_66_1) 1989 e_1_2_8_8_1 e_1_2_8_21_1 e_1_2_8_42_1 e_1_2_8_23_1 e_1_2_8_44_1 e_1_2_8_65_1 e_1_2_8_86_1 Schneider W. (e_1_2_8_74_1) 2002 e_1_2_8_63_1 e_1_2_8_84_1 Fillmore C. J. (e_1_2_8_18_1) 1968 e_1_2_8_61_1 e_1_2_8_82_1 Golinkoff R. M. (e_1_2_8_27_1) 1978; 24 e_1_2_8_14_1 e_1_2_8_35_1 e_1_2_8_16_1 e_1_2_8_37_1 Jackendoff R. (e_1_2_8_41_1) 1996 e_1_2_8_58_1 e_1_2_8_79_1 Gleitman L. (e_1_2_8_26_1) 2016 Landau B. (e_1_2_8_51_1) 2010 Grosz B. (e_1_2_8_34_1) 1995; 21 Whorf B. L. (e_1_2_8_81_1) 1956 e_1_2_8_10_1 e_1_2_8_31_1 e_1_2_8_56_1 Core Team R (e_1_2_8_67_1) 2018 e_1_2_8_77_1 e_1_2_8_12_1 e_1_2_8_54_1 e_1_2_8_75_1 e_1_2_8_52_1 e_1_2_8_50_1 e_1_2_8_71_1 |
References_xml | – volume: 66 start-page: 554 year: 2016 end-page: 580 article-title: Interactions between language and mental representations publication-title: Language Learning – volume: 19 start-page: 441 year: 1987 end-page: 472 article-title: Adapting production to comprehension: The explicit mention of instruments publication-title: Cognitive Psychology – volume: 7 start-page: 287 year: 2003 end-page: 292 article-title: Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naive theory of rational action publication-title: Trends in Cognitive Sciences – volume: 21 start-page: 203 issue: 2 year: 1995 end-page: 225 article-title: Centering: a framework for modelling the local coherence of discourse publication-title: Computational Linguistics – year: 2005 – volume: 25 start-page: 265 year: 1987 end-page: 288 article-title: Do six‐month‐old infants perceive causality? publication-title: Cognition – start-page: 51 year: 2010 end-page: 78 – volume: 29 start-page: 421 year: 1993 end-page: 433 article-title: How infants perceive a simple causal event publication-title: Developmental Psychology – start-page: 15 year: 2009 end-page: 48 – volume: 31 start-page: 705 year: 2007 end-page: 719 article-title: Attention to endpoints: A cross‐linguistic constraint on spatial meaning publication-title: Cognitive Science – volume: 13 start-page: 224 year: 2021 end-page: 242 article-title: From event representation to linguistic meaning publication-title: Topics in Cognitive Science – volume: 125 start-page: 129 year: 2007 end-page: 143 article-title: Describing scenes hardly seen publication-title: Acta Psychologica – volume: 34 start-page: 826 year: 2019 end-page: 840 article-title: The source‐goal asymmetry in spatial language: Language‐general vs. language‐specific aspects publication-title: Language, Cognition and Neuroscience – year: 1989 – volume: 4 start-page: 177 year: 2009 end-page: 184 article-title: Using language to navigate the infant mind publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science – volume: 430 start-page: 453 year: 2004 end-page: 456 article-title: Conceptual precursors to language publication-title: Nature – volume: 164 start-page: 174 year: 2017 end-page: 187 article-title: The relationship between pre‐verbal event representations and semantic structures: The case of goal and source paths publication-title: Cognition – volume: 41 start-page: 70 year: 2017 end-page: 101 article-title: Language reflects “core” cognition: A new theory about the origin of cross‐linguistic regularities publication-title: Cognitive Science – volume: 57 start-page: 544 year: 2007 end-page: 569 article-title: On the give and take between event apprehension and utterance formulation publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 127 start-page: 3 year: 2001 end-page: 21 article-title: Event structure in perception and conception publication-title: Psychological Bulletin – volume: 11 start-page: 274 year: 2000 end-page: 279 article-title: What the eyes say about speaking publication-title: Psychological Science – year: 1990 – year: 2018 – year: 2002a – start-page: 35 year: 1956 end-page: 278 – year: 2014 – start-page: 249 year: 2004 end-page: 278 – volume: 66 start-page: B25 year: 1998 end-page: B33 article-title: Viewing and naming objects: Eye movements during noun phrase production publication-title: Cognition – volume: 43 year: 2019b article-title: Children's (and Adults') production adjustments to generic and particular listener needs publication-title: Cognitive Science – volume: 56 start-page: 165 year: 1995 end-page: 193 article-title: Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age publication-title: Cognition – volume: 142 start-page: 266 year: 2015 end-page: 290 article-title: Using instruments to understand argument structure: Evidence for gradient representation publication-title: Cognition – start-page: 1 year: 1996 end-page: 30 – volume: 69 start-page: 1 year: 1998 end-page: 34 article-title: Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach publication-title: Cognition – year: 1997 – volume: 36 start-page: 517 year: 2012 end-page: 544 article-title: Language and memory for motion events: Origins of the asymmetry between source and goal paths publication-title: Cognitive Science – volume: 34 start-page: 1117 year: 2019 end-page: 1128 article-title: The role of conceptualization during language production: Evidence from event encoding publication-title: Language, Cognition and Neuroscience – volume: 55 start-page: 951 year: 2019a end-page: 966 article-title: Interactive contexts increase informativeness in children's referential communication publication-title: Developmental Psychology – volume: 41 start-page: 1 year: 1991 end-page: 7 article-title: Introduction to special issue of cognition on lexical and conceptual semantics publication-title: Cognition – volume: 48 start-page: 16 year: 2003 end-page: 32 article-title: What does cross‐linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking publication-title: Journal of Memory and Language – volume: 26 start-page: 1850 year: 2019 end-page: 1869 article-title: Thematic roles: Core knowledge or linguistic construct? publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 67 start-page: 1 issue: 1 year: 2015 end-page: 48 article-title: Fitting linear mixed‐effects models using lme4 publication-title: Journal of Statistical Software – year: 2019 – year: 1983 – volume: 96 start-page: 1 year: 2005 end-page: 33 article-title: Starting at the end: The importance of goals in spatial language publication-title: Cognition – volume: 32 start-page: 99 year: 1989 end-page: 156 article-title: Mechanisms that improve referential access publication-title: Cognition – volume: 205 year: 2020 article-title: Cognitive and pragmatic factors in language production: Evidence from source‐goal motion events publication-title: Cognition – volume: 183 start-page: 152 year: 2019 end-page: 180 article-title: Why wait for the verb? Turkish speaking children use case markers for incremental language comprehension publication-title: Cognition – volume: 24 start-page: 53 year: 1978 end-page: 61 article-title: Infants' perception of semantically defined action role changes in filmed events publication-title: Merrill‐Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development – volume: 32 start-page: 5795 year: 2012 end-page: 5803 article-title: The effect of object‐state changes on event processing: Do objects compete with themselves? publication-title: The Journal of Neuroscience – year: 2003 – volume: 25 start-page: 884 year: 2015 end-page: 895 article-title: A cortical network for the encoding of object change publication-title: Cerebral Cortex – volume: 67 start-page: 547 year: 1991 end-page: 619 article-title: Thematic proto‐roles and argument selection publication-title: Language – start-page: 917 year: 2006 end-page: 921 – volume: 16 start-page: 995 issue: 12 year: 2005 end-page: 1001 article-title: Secret agents: Inferences about hidden causes by 10‐and 12‐month‐old infants publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 41 start-page: 83 year: 1991 end-page: 121 article-title: Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language‐specific lexicalization patterns publication-title: Cognition – year: 2016 – start-page: 165 year: 1981 end-page: 82 – volume: 5 start-page: 33 year: 2010 end-page: 42 article-title: Trading Spaces: Carving up events for learning language publication-title: Perspectives on Psychological Science – volume: 142 start-page: 880 year: 2013 end-page: 905 article-title: Getting the gist of events: Recognition of two‐participant actions from brief displays publication-title: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General – volume: 31 start-page: 1015 year: 2016 end-page: 1037 article-title: How children and adults encode causative events cross‐linguistically: Implications for language production and attention publication-title: Language, Cognition and Neuroscience – year: 1984 – volume: 9 start-page: 550 year: 2002 end-page: 557 article-title: Addressees’ needs influence speakers’ early syntactic choices publication-title: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review – volume: 136 start-page: 82 year: 2007 end-page: 111 article-title: Representing causation publication-title: Journal of Experiment Psychology:General – start-page: 1206 year: 2011 end-page: 1211 article-title: Rapid extraction of event participants in caused motion events – volume: 3 start-page: 179 year: 2007 end-page: 197 article-title: Conceptual foundations of spatial language: Evidence for a goal bias in infants publication-title: Language Learning and Development – volume: 122 start-page: 135 year: 2012 end-page: 149 article-title: The relation between event apprehension and utterance formulation in children: Evidence from linguistic omissions publication-title: Cognition – volume: 8 start-page: 368 year: 1997 end-page: 373 article-title: To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes publication-title: Psychological Science – volume: 72 start-page: 708 issue: 3 year: 2001 end-page: 717 article-title: Infants parse dynamic action publication-title: Child Development – volume: 15 start-page: 138 year: 2019 end-page: 156 article-title: How children identify events from visual experience publication-title: Language, Learning and Development – start-page: 1 year: 1968 end-page: 88 – start-page: 73 year: 1997 end-page: 137 – volume: 11 start-page: 152 year: 2015 end-page: 175 article-title: Twelve‐month‐old infants’ encoding of goal and source paths in agentive and non‐agentive motion events publication-title: Language Learning and Development – volume: 25 start-page: 224 year: 2010 end-page: 260 article-title: Event categorisation and language: A cross‐linguistic study of motion publication-title: Language and Cognitive Processes – year: 2002b – volume: 175 start-page: 36 year: 2018 end-page: 52 article-title: Encoding of event roles from visual scenes is rapid, spontaneous, and interacts with higher‐level visual processing publication-title: Cognition – volume: 102 start-page: 16 year: 2007 end-page: 48 article-title: Language‐specific and universal influences in children's syntactic packaging of manner and path: A comparison of English, Japanese, and Turkish publication-title: Cognition – volume: 29 start-page: 620 year: 2014 end-page: 634 article-title: Early language‐specificity of children's event encoding in speech and gesture: Evidence from caused motion in Turkish. publication-title: Cognition and Neuroscience – volume: 1 start-page: 3 year: 1990 end-page: 55 article-title: The structural sources of verb learning publication-title: Language Acquisition – volume: 1 start-page: 399 year: 2011 end-page: 420 – volume: 84 start-page: 189 year: 2002 end-page: 219 article-title: Shake, rattle,‘n'roll: The representation of motion in language and cognition publication-title: Cognition – volume: 98 start-page: B75 year: 2006 end-page: B87 article-title: When English proposes what Greek presupposes: The cross‐linguistic encoding of motion events publication-title: Cognition – volume: 34 start-page: 1064 year: 2010 end-page: 1092 article-title: Source‐goal asymmetries in motion representation: Implications for language production and comprehension publication-title: Cognitive Science – ident: e_1_2_8_62_1 doi: 10.1080/23273798.2019.1589540 – ident: e_1_2_8_84_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_37_1 doi: 10.1038/nature02634 – volume-title: Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning year: 2016 ident: e_1_2_8_26_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_59_1 doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00009-2 – ident: e_1_2_8_3_1 doi: 10.1007/978-94-011-5420-8_2 – ident: e_1_2_8_14_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104447 – ident: e_1_2_8_35_1 doi: 10.1037/a0030045 – ident: e_1_2_8_46_1 doi: 10.1080/15475441.2014.896168 – start-page: 1 volume-title: Language and space year: 1996 ident: e_1_2_8_41_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_21_1 doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00128-1 – ident: e_1_2_8_15_1 doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.07.004 – ident: e_1_2_8_58_1 doi: 10.3758/BF03196312 – ident: e_1_2_8_44_1 doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00505-3 – ident: e_1_2_8_87_1 doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.3 – volume-title: The function of word order in Turkish grammar year: 1984 ident: e_1_2_8_17_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_30_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00255 – ident: e_1_2_8_86_1 doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4 – ident: e_1_2_8_42_1 doi: 10.1080/23273798.2019.1584323 – ident: e_1_2_8_4_1 doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00310 – ident: e_1_2_8_56_1 doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90030-8 – ident: e_1_2_8_63_1 doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00046-X – ident: e_1_2_8_70_1 doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01634-5 – ident: e_1_2_8_24_1 doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0101_2 – ident: e_1_2_8_29_1 doi: 10.1177/1745691609356783 – ident: e_1_2_8_54_1 doi: 10.1093/obo/9780199772810-0141 – volume-title: E‐prime reference guide year: 2002 ident: e_1_2_8_74_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_79_1 doi: 10.1111/tops.12475 – ident: e_1_2_8_11_1 doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90033-Z – volume: 21 start-page: 203 issue: 2 year: 1995 ident: e_1_2_8_34_1 article-title: Centering: a framework for modelling the local coherence of discourse publication-title: Computational Linguistics – start-page: 51 volume-title: Language, cognition, and space: The state of the art and new directions year: 2010 ident: e_1_2_8_51_1 – start-page: 249 volume-title: The interface of language, vision, and action: Eye movements and the visual world year: 2004 ident: e_1_2_8_6_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_85_1 doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.82 – start-page: 1 volume-title: Universals in linguistic theory year: 1968 ident: e_1_2_8_18_1 – start-page: 165 volume-title: The logical problem of language acquisition year: 1981 ident: e_1_2_8_33_1 – volume-title: Semantics and cognition year: 1983 ident: e_1_2_8_39_1 – volume-title: E‐prime user's guide year: 2002 ident: e_1_2_8_73_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_16_1 doi: 10.1353/lan.1991.0021 – ident: e_1_2_8_10_1 doi: 10.1080/23273798.2016.1175649 – ident: e_1_2_8_38_1 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6294-11.2012 – ident: e_1_2_8_12_1 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.29.3.421 – ident: e_1_2_8_2_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.12.006 – ident: e_1_2_8_13_1 doi: 10.1515/9783110226614.399 – volume-title: R: A language and environment for statistical computing year: 2018 ident: e_1_2_8_67_1 – start-page: 35 volume-title: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf year: 1956 ident: e_1_2_8_81_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_82_1 doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-24277-4 – ident: e_1_2_8_8_1 doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(87)90015-6 – ident: e_1_2_8_7_1 doi: 10.1002/0470018860.s00567 – ident: e_1_2_8_52_1 doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(87)80006-9 – ident: e_1_2_8_57_1 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511613609 – ident: e_1_2_8_69_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x – ident: e_1_2_8_60_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.026 – ident: e_1_2_8_20_1 doi: 10.1080/01690965.2013.824993 – ident: e_1_2_8_31_1 doi: 10.1037/dev0000693 – ident: e_1_2_8_32_1 doi: 10.1111/cogs.12790 – ident: e_1_2_8_72_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01649.x – ident: e_1_2_8_76_1 doi: 10.1111/cogs.12332 – volume-title: Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure year: 1989 ident: e_1_2_8_66_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_65_1 doi: 10.1080/01690960903017000 – ident: e_1_2_8_25_1 doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.007 – volume-title: Turkish year: 1997 ident: e_1_2_8_45_1 – volume: 24 start-page: 53 year: 1978 ident: e_1_2_8_27_1 article-title: Infants' perception of semantically defined action role changes in filmed events publication-title: Merrill‐Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development – ident: e_1_2_8_71_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.015 – ident: e_1_2_8_22_1 doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00661-H – volume-title: Speaking year: 1989 ident: e_1_2_8_53_1 doi: 10.7551/mitpress/6393.001.0001 – volume-title: Semantic structures year: 1990 ident: e_1_2_8_40_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_43_1 doi: 10.1515/9783110217117.15 – ident: e_1_2_8_5_1 doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 – ident: e_1_2_8_23_1 doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(89)90001-2 – ident: e_1_2_8_77_1 doi: 10.1111/lang.12188 – ident: e_1_2_8_50_1 doi: 10.1080/15475440701360168 – start-page: 1206 volume-title: Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society year: 2011 ident: e_1_2_8_83_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_75_1 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht275 – ident: e_1_2_8_61_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01107.x – ident: e_1_2_8_55_1 doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511610479 – ident: e_1_2_8_19_1 – ident: e_1_2_8_68_1 doi: 10.1080/15326900701399954 – ident: e_1_2_8_49_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.04.003 – ident: e_1_2_8_78_1 doi: 10.1080/15475441.2018.1544075 – ident: e_1_2_8_9_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.002 – ident: e_1_2_8_47_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.03.009 – ident: e_1_2_8_28_1 doi: 10.4324/9780203340769 – ident: e_1_2_8_48_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01220.x – ident: e_1_2_8_64_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.05.005 – ident: e_1_2_8_36_1 doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.02.011 – ident: e_1_2_8_80_1 doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01117.x |
SSID | ssj0004301 |
Score | 2.4348497 |
Snippet | Although it is widely assumed that the linguistic description of events is based on a structured representation of event components at the... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed eric crossref wiley |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | e13116 |
SubjectTerms | Accuracy Blindness causative events change blindness Child, Preschool Cognition Cognition & reasoning cross‐linguistic differences English event cognition event perception Goals Homology Humans Language Linguistics Perception Preschool Children Proxy thematic roles Turkish |
Title | Representing agents, patients, goals and instruments in causative events: A cross‐linguistic investigation of early language and cognition |
URI | https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111%2Fdesc.13116 http://eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/detail?accno=EJ1314959 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/33955664 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2583449605 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2522620641 |
Volume | 24 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
journalDatabaseRights | – providerCode: PRVEBS databaseName: EBSCOhost Academic Search Ultimate customDbUrl: https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?authtype=ip,shib&custid=s3936755&profile=ehost&defaultdb=asn eissn: 1467-7687 dateEnd: 20241003 omitProxy: true ssIdentifier: ssj0004301 issn: 1363-755X databaseCode: ABDBF dateStart: 19980401 isFulltext: true titleUrlDefault: https://search.ebscohost.com/direct.asp?db=asn providerName: EBSCOhost – providerCode: PRVWIB databaseName: Wiley Online Library - Core collection (SURFmarket) issn: 1363-755X databaseCode: DR2 dateStart: 19980101 customDbUrl: isFulltext: true eissn: 1467-7687 dateEnd: 99991231 omitProxy: false ssIdentifier: ssj0004301 providerName: Wiley-Blackwell |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3LbtUwEB1VXXVTXi0ESmVENyBylcRxEiM2VblVVaks-pDuBkWOY1cIlCCldwErPoAF38iXMDN5tBehSmVnJRMrcc7YM_bMGYC9orCxNLENZeKJVFvJ0OgCHVdVo3le2STylO988iE7ukiPF2qxBu_GXJieH2LacCPN4PmaFNxU3Q0lr11nZ0QWQ3zbsVR8Rnt6zR2VymhIupJhrtRi4CalMJ7rR1dWo9WA5xuW5qrhyivP4T34OL5zH3Dyeba8qmb2-190jv_7UfdhczBJxX6PoQew5pqHsDHNjN8ewc9Tjpd1XFVCGMrG6t6IgZIVW5ctgliYphafmJCW0-awLaxZdswsLpgoqnsr9gWPxO8fvygPfsk00Sg5kX20jWi9cES7LMa9VO55iHNqmy24OJyfHxyFQxmH0MpcZ2FqDPPIOevySufWIAzqOsalkShJpfORLmLvNNqqMonSSkqcdWwW5YiV2qKzvw3rTdu4JyBMEvvMR5FzWqbKYe8m1oXMdOWdd3kWwKvxd5Z24DinUhtfytHXoQEueYADeDnJfu2ZPf4ptU2omCTmx3gZnUodwM6Ik3JQ-65MFJUtQadQBfBiuo0KS6cwpnHtkmQSKgKQpXEAj3t8Tb1LqRXa12kArxklt7xY-X5-dsCtp3cRfgYbCUXlcDblDqwjJtxzNKuuql1Wn13eQPgD5b4gAQ |
linkProvider | Wiley-Blackwell |
linkToHtml | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnZ3NbtQwEIBHUA70wn9LoIARXEBklcRxEnOr2q2W0vZQWmlvkePYCIGSSukeyokH4MAz8iTMTLJpFyEkuFm7s9aud8aecWa-AXhZFDaWJrahTDxBtZUMjS4wcFU1uueVTSJP9c6HR9nsNN2fq_mQm0O1MD0fYrxwI8vg_ZoMnC6kr1h57To7IVpMdh1u0AM6ssvd40t6VCqjoexKhrlS84FOSok8l59dOY9WU56v-JqrriufPXu3-warHSMLKeXk82RxXk3s19-Ajv_9s-7ArcErFdu9Gt2Fa665B-vj5nhxH74fc8qs48YSwlBBVvdGDFRWHH1sUY-FaWrxiZm0XDmHY2HNomO4uGBWVPdWbAteip_fflAp_IJJ0Sg58j7aRrReOCIvi-V1Ks88pDq1zQM43Zue7MzCoZNDaGWuszA1hlFyzrq80rk1qAl1HePpSFRS6Xyki9g7je6qTKK0khI3HptFOapLbTHe34C1pm3cQxAmiX3mo8g5LVPlcHYT60JmuvLOuzwL4NXy_yztgDmnbhtfymW4Qwtc8gIH8GKUPevhHn-U2iC1GCWm-_gyxpU6gK2lopSD5XdloqhzCcaFKoDn49tos_QgxjSuXZBMQn0AsjQOYLNXsHF2KbVCFzsN4DWryV--WLk7_bDDo0f_IvwMbs5ODg_Kg3dH7x_DekJJOlxcuQVrqB_uCXpZ59VTtqVf_VsjHA |
linkToPdf | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3NbtQwEB6VIqFe-C-kFDCCC4iskjh2YsSlandVClSoUGkvVeQ4NkJUSaV0D3DiATj0GfskzDg_7SKEBDcrmViO840948x8A_Asz03MdWxCnjgi1RY81CpHx1VUaJ6XJokc5Tu_35e7h-neXMxX4PWQC9PxQ4wHbqQZfr0mBT-p3CUlr2xrJkQWI6_A1VSie0Um0cEFeVTKoz7rioeZEPOenJTieC6eXdqOliOeL5may5ar33pmN-BoGHQXcfJ1sjgtJ-b7b3yO__tWN-F6b5OyrQ5Et2DF1rdhbVwav92Bnwc-YNb6shJMUzpW-5L1nKzY-twgipmuK_bFM9L6vDlsM6MXracWZ54pqn3FtpififMfZ5QIv_A80Sg5sn00NWscs8S7zIbDVN9zH-jU1HfhcDb9tL0b9nUcQsMzJcNUa08kZ43NSpUZjTioqhj3RuIk5dZFKo-dVWis8iRKS85x2TEyyhAslUFvfx1W66a294HpJHbSRZG1iqfCYu86VjmXqnTW2UwG8Hz4nIXpSc6p1sZxMTg7NMGFn-AAno6yJx21xx-l1gkVo8R0Dy-jV6kC2BxwUvR63xaJoLol6BWKAJ6Mt1Fj6TeMrm2zIJmEqgDINA7gXoevsXfOlUADOw3ghUfJXwZW7Ew_bvvWxr8IP4ZrH3Zmxbs3-28fwFpCETo-s3ITVhEe9iGaWKflI69JvwCBFiHL |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Representing+agents%2C+patients%2C+goals+and+instruments+in+causative+events%3A+A+cross-linguistic+investigation+of+early+language+and+cognition&rft.jtitle=Developmental+science&rft.au=%C3%9Cnal%2C+Ercenur&rft.au=Richards%2C+Catherine&rft.au=Trueswell%2C+John+C&rft.au=Papafragou%2C+Anna&rft.date=2021-11-01&rft.eissn=1467-7687&rft.volume=24&rft.issue=6&rft.spage=e13116&rft_id=info:doi/10.1111%2Fdesc.13116&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F33955664&rft.externalDocID=33955664 |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1363-755X&client=summon |