Testing a positive-affect induction to reduce verbally induced nocebo hyperalgesia in an experimental pain paradigm
There is an ethical obligation to notify individuals about potential pain associated with diagnoses, treatments, and procedures; however, supplying this information risks inducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Currently, there are few empirically derived strategies for reducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Because n...
Saved in:
Published in | Pain (Amsterdam) Vol. 160; no. 10; pp. 2290 - 2297 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , , , , , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
Wolters Kluwer
01.10.2019
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 0304-3959 1872-6623 1872-6623 |
DOI | 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001618 |
Cover
Abstract | There is an ethical obligation to notify individuals about potential pain associated with diagnoses, treatments, and procedures; however, supplying this information risks inducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Currently, there are few empirically derived strategies for reducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Because nocebo effects are linked to negative affectivity, we tested the hypothesis that a positive-affect induction can disrupt nocebo hyperalgesia from verbal suggestion. Healthy volunteers (N = 147) were randomly assigned to conditions in a 2 (affect induction: positive vs neutral) by 2 (verbal suggestion: no suggestion vs suggestion of pain increase) between-subjects design. Participants were induced to experience positive or neutral affect by watching movie clips for 15 minutes. Next, participants had an inert cream applied to their nondominant hand, and suggestion was manipulated by telling only half the participants the cream could increase the pain of the upcoming cold pressor test. Subsequently, all participants underwent the cold pressor test (8 ± 0.04°C), wherein they submerged the nondominant hand and rated pain intensity on numerical rating scales every 20 seconds up to 2 minutes. In the neutral-affect conditions, there was evidence for the nocebo hyperalgesia effect: participants given the suggestion of pain displayed greater pain than participants not receiving this suggestion,
P
's < 0.05. Demonstrating a blockage effect, nocebo hyperalgesia did not occur in the positive-affect conditions,
P
's > 0.5. This is the first study to show that positive affect may disrupt nocebo hyperalgesia thereby pointing to a novel strategy for decreasing nocebo effects without compromising the communication of medical information to patients in clinical settings. |
---|---|
AbstractList | There is an ethical obligation to notify individuals about potential pain associated with diagnoses, treatments, and procedures; however, supplying this information risks inducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Currently, there are few empirically derived strategies for reducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Because nocebo effects are linked to negative affectivity, we tested the hypothesis that a positive-affect induction can disrupt nocebo hyperalgesia from verbal suggestion. Healthy volunteers (N = 147) were randomly assigned to conditions in a 2 (affect induction: positive vs neutral) by 2 (verbal suggestion: no suggestion vs suggestion of pain increase) between-subjects design. Participants were induced to experience positive or neutral affect by watching movie clips for 15 minutes. Next, participants had an inert cream applied to their nondominant hand, and suggestion was manipulated by telling only half the participants the cream could increase the pain of the upcoming cold pressor test. Subsequently, all participants underwent the cold pressor test (8 ± 0.04°C), wherein they submerged the nondominant hand and rated pain intensity on numerical rating scales every 20 seconds up to 2 minutes. In the neutral-affect conditions, there was evidence for the nocebo hyperalgesia effect: participants given the suggestion of pain displayed greater pain than participants not receiving this suggestion, P's < 0.05. Demonstrating a blockage effect, nocebo hyperalgesia did not occur in the positive-affect conditions, P's > 0.5. This is the first study to show that positive affect may disrupt nocebo hyperalgesia thereby pointing to a novel strategy for decreasing nocebo effects without compromising the communication of medical information to patients in clinical settings. There is an ethical obligation to notify individuals about potential pain associated with diagnoses, treatments, and procedures; however, supplying this information risks inducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Currently, there are few empirically derived strategies for reducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Because nocebo effects are linked to negative affectivity, we tested the hypothesis that a positive-affect induction can disrupt nocebo hyperalgesia from verbal suggestion. Healthy volunteers (N = 147) were randomly assigned to conditions in a 2 (affect induction: positive vs neutral) by 2 (verbal suggestion: no suggestion vs suggestion of pain increase) between-subjects design. Participants were induced to experience positive or neutral affect by watching movie clips for 15 minutes. Next, participants had an inert cream applied to their nondominant hand, and suggestion was manipulated by telling only half the participants the cream could increase the pain of the upcoming cold pressor test. Subsequently, all participants underwent the cold pressor test (8 ± 0.04°C), wherein they submerged the nondominant hand and rated pain intensity on numerical rating scales every 20 seconds up to 2 minutes. In the neutral-affect conditions, there was evidence for the nocebo hyperalgesia effect: participants given the suggestion of pain displayed greater pain than participants not receiving this suggestion, P's < 0.05. Demonstrating a blockage effect, nocebo hyperalgesia did not occur in the positive-affect conditions, P's > 0.5. This is the first study to show that positive affect may disrupt nocebo hyperalgesia thereby pointing to a novel strategy for decreasing nocebo effects without compromising the communication of medical information to patients in clinical settings.There is an ethical obligation to notify individuals about potential pain associated with diagnoses, treatments, and procedures; however, supplying this information risks inducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Currently, there are few empirically derived strategies for reducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Because nocebo effects are linked to negative affectivity, we tested the hypothesis that a positive-affect induction can disrupt nocebo hyperalgesia from verbal suggestion. Healthy volunteers (N = 147) were randomly assigned to conditions in a 2 (affect induction: positive vs neutral) by 2 (verbal suggestion: no suggestion vs suggestion of pain increase) between-subjects design. Participants were induced to experience positive or neutral affect by watching movie clips for 15 minutes. Next, participants had an inert cream applied to their nondominant hand, and suggestion was manipulated by telling only half the participants the cream could increase the pain of the upcoming cold pressor test. Subsequently, all participants underwent the cold pressor test (8 ± 0.04°C), wherein they submerged the nondominant hand and rated pain intensity on numerical rating scales every 20 seconds up to 2 minutes. In the neutral-affect conditions, there was evidence for the nocebo hyperalgesia effect: participants given the suggestion of pain displayed greater pain than participants not receiving this suggestion, P's < 0.05. Demonstrating a blockage effect, nocebo hyperalgesia did not occur in the positive-affect conditions, P's > 0.5. This is the first study to show that positive affect may disrupt nocebo hyperalgesia thereby pointing to a novel strategy for decreasing nocebo effects without compromising the communication of medical information to patients in clinical settings. There is an ethical obligation to notify individuals about potential pain associated with diagnoses, treatments, and procedures; however, supplying this information risks inducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Currently, there are few empirically derived strategies for reducing nocebo hyperalgesia. Because nocebo effects are linked to negative affectivity, we tested the hypothesis that a positive-affect induction can disrupt nocebo hyperalgesia from verbal suggestion. Healthy volunteers (N = 147) were randomly assigned to conditions in a 2 (affect induction: positive vs neutral) by 2 (verbal suggestion: no suggestion vs suggestion of pain increase) between-subjects design. Participants were induced to experience positive or neutral affect by watching movie clips for 15 minutes. Next, participants had an inert cream applied to their nondominant hand, and suggestion was manipulated by telling only half the participants the cream could increase the pain of the upcoming cold pressor test. Subsequently, all participants underwent the cold pressor test (8 ± 0.04°C), wherein they submerged the nondominant hand and rated pain intensity on numerical rating scales every 20 seconds up to 2 minutes. In the neutral-affect conditions, there was evidence for the nocebo hyperalgesia effect: participants given the suggestion of pain displayed greater pain than participants not receiving this suggestion, P 's < 0.05. Demonstrating a blockage effect, nocebo hyperalgesia did not occur in the positive-affect conditions, P 's > 0.5. This is the first study to show that positive affect may disrupt nocebo hyperalgesia thereby pointing to a novel strategy for decreasing nocebo effects without compromising the communication of medical information to patients in clinical settings. |
Author | Close, Shane Vase, Lene Handley, Ian M. Caplandies, Fawn C. Murray, Ashley B. Vogel, Charles L. Pertiwi, Yopina Geers, Andrew L. |
AuthorAffiliation | Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, United States Department of Psychology, Montana State University, Bozeman, MT, United States Department of Psychology and Behavioural Sciences, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark |
AuthorAffiliation_xml | – name: Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, United States – name: Department of Psychology, Montana State University, Bozeman, MT, United States – name: Department of Psychology and Behavioural Sciences, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Andrew L. surname: Geers fullname: Geers, Andrew L. organization: Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, United States – sequence: 2 givenname: Shane surname: Close fullname: Close, Shane organization: Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, United States – sequence: 3 givenname: Fawn C. surname: Caplandies fullname: Caplandies, Fawn C. organization: Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, United States – sequence: 4 givenname: Charles L. surname: Vogel fullname: Vogel, Charles L. organization: Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, United States – sequence: 5 givenname: Ashley B. surname: Murray fullname: Murray, Ashley B. organization: Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, United States – sequence: 6 givenname: Yopina surname: Pertiwi fullname: Pertiwi, Yopina organization: Department of Psychology, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH, United States – sequence: 7 givenname: Ian M. surname: Handley fullname: Handley, Ian M. organization: Department of Psychology, Montana State University, Bozeman, MT, United States – sequence: 8 givenname: Lene surname: Vase fullname: Vase, Lene organization: Department of Psychology and Behavioural Sciences, Aarhus University Hospital, Aarhus, Denmark |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31107412$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNqNkU1v1DAQhi1URLeFvwA-csnisbNJfOCAKqBIlbiUs-U4k10vXjvYTtv99zjdglBPzMGjmXleaz4uyJkPHgl5B2wNTLYf9utJW79m_xg00L0gK-haXjUNF2dkxQSrKyE38pxcpLQvEOdcviLnAoC1NfAVSbeYsvVbqukUks32Dis9jmgytX6YTbbB0xxoxBIgvcPYa-eOpyIO1AeDfaC744RRuy0mq0uNak_xoaTsAX3Wji7dlifqwW4Pr8nLUbuEb578Jfnx5fPt1XV18_3rt6tPN5URm05U_QaQb7qWgeiEBK5rHJgcmpELI4dRjLyHoTZQEGQSSlpoHCXrZctHpkFckvenf6cYfs1lTnWwyaBz2mOYk-JccNZ0oqsL-vYJnfsDDmoqnet4VH8WVYD2BJgYUoo4_kWAqeUkaq-WIdXzkxTlx2dKY7Ne9pqjtu4_9PVJfx9cxph-uvkeo9qhdnn3iDdCNhVnIGGJqkUoxG_SKaOW |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_1016_j_tics_2021_07_016 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jenvp_2023_102095 crossref_primary_10_3390_ijerph18084206 crossref_primary_10_1177_13591053231186050 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_jpain_2021_02_008 crossref_primary_10_1111_spc3_12575 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.10.013 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1976.tb01330.x 10.1037/1082-989X.6.4.330 10.1080/19312458.2011.624490 10.1016/j.pain.2008.02.006 10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.539 10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.356 10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147 10.1197/aemj.10.4.390 10.1080/026999398379718 10.1038/nrn2333 10.1002/cpt.31 10.1155/2005/216481 10.1159/000490354 10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.337 10.1016/j.pain.2004.08.013 10.1097/PR9.0000000000000586 10.1007/s10865-010-9291-7 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000940 10.1097/ACO.0b013e3282b972fb 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182294a50 10.1177/0146167210384710 10.2147/JPR.S91923 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.010 10.1002/pds.4169 10.1001/jama.287.5.622 10.1023/A:1021725130325 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00081-0 10.1037/a0028262 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.11.003 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199607)26:4<557::AID-EJSP769>3.0.CO;2-4 10.1016/j.pain.2013.07.013 10.1016/j.pain.2012.02.042 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.02.020 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)03346-0 10.1177/1094428114548590 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00073-3 10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.1034 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2191-13.2013 10.1007/s10865-011-9374-0 10.1037/0022-3514.72.5.1017 10.1126/scitranslmed.3001244 10.1037/a0034005 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530 10.1016/S0895-4356(96)00177-1 10.1007/s12529-018-9726-z 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.34 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2944-08.2008 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00352.x 10.1037/h0036128 10.1080/01973533.2013.803968 10.1037/0022-3514.43.1.154 10.1080/02699930143000266 10.3758/BF03193146 10.1037/hea0000416 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.12.006 10.1007/s10865-015-9708-4 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.02.014 10.1007/BF01190070 10.1016/j.pain.2014.04.016 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00086-6 10.1037/0022-3514.37.5.688 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2012.01950.x 10.1007/s10865-014-9586-1 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.08.004 10.1016/j.pain.0000000000000004 10.1037/hea0000199 10.1126/science.aap8488 10.1006/pmed.1996.0124 10.1016/j.pain.2013.04.041 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00016-X 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(200003/04)30:2<211::AID-EJSP988>3.0.CO;2-3 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.015 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000840 10.1007/BF00991573 10.1037/emo0000035 10.1016/j.pain.2004.08.008 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2005.01121.x 10.1098/rstb.2010.0407 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.08.006 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
Copyright | Wolters Kluwer |
Copyright_xml | – notice: Wolters Kluwer |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001618 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic CrossRef |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine |
EISSN | 1872-6623 |
EndPage | 2297 |
ExternalDocumentID | 31107412 10_1097_j_pain_0000000000001618 00006396-201910000-00013 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- --K 026 0R~ 123 1B1 1~5 4.4 71M AAAAV AAAXR AAGIX AAHPQ AAIQE AAMOA AAQKA AARTV AASCR AASXQ AAUEB AAXQO ABASU ABBUW ABDIG ABIVO ABJNI ABLJU ABOCM ABPXF ABVCZ ABXVJ ABZAD ABZZY ACDDN ACDOF ACEWG ACGFO ACGFS ACILI ACLDA ACNWC ACOAL ACWDW ACWRI ACXJB ACXNZ ACZKN ADGGA ADHPY AEKER AENEX AFBFQ AFDTB AFMBP AFSOK AGGSO AHOMT AHQNM AHVBC AHXIK AIJEX AINUH AJCLO AJIOK AJNWD AJZMW AKCTQ AKRWK AKULP ALKUP ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS ALMTX AMJPA AMKUR AMNEI AOHHW AOQMC BOYCO BQLVK BYPQX C45 CS3 DIWNM DU5 EBS EEVPB ERAAH EX3 F5P FCALG FDB GNXGY GQDEL HLJTE HMQ HZ~ IHE IKREB IKYAY L-C L7B MJL MO0 N9A O-L O9- OBH OPUJH OVD OVDNE OVIDH OVLEI OVOZU OXXIT OZT P2P RLZ RPZ SCC SEL SES TEORI TSPGW TWZ .55 .GJ .~1 1CY 1RT 1~. 29O 3O- 4G. 53G 5VS 7-5 9JO AABNK AAGUQ AAIKJ AALRI AAQFI AAQQT AAXUO AAYWO AAYXX ABBQC ABCQJ ABFNM ABMAC ABZDS ACIUM ACJTP ACXNI ADBBV ADNKB AEETU AERZD AFXBA AGWIK AGYEJ AHHHB AIGII AITUG AJNYG AJRQY AKBMS AKYEP ALCLG AMRAJ CITATION DUNZO EJD EO8 EO9 EP2 EP3 FEDTE FGOYB FIRID FNPLU G-2 G-Q HDV HMK HMO HVGLF H~9 IPNFZ J1W J5H LX1 M29 M2V M41 OHT OUVQU P-8 P-9 PC. Q38 R2- RIG ROL SAE SDF SDG SDP SEW SNS SSZ WUQ X7M XPP ZGI ZZMQN CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 ADKSD ADSXY |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c3583-b51e258701383912a4ed09d6f23c9df3f2b1d4c1587e091f233aef90b972f0a13 |
ISSN | 0304-3959 1872-6623 |
IngestDate | Mon Sep 08 02:57:07 EDT 2025 Mon Jul 21 05:59:15 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 02:34:25 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:09:03 EDT 2025 Fri May 16 03:50:36 EDT 2025 |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 10 |
Language | English |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c3583-b51e258701383912a4ed09d6f23c9df3f2b1d4c1587e091f233aef90b972f0a13 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
PMID | 31107412 |
PQID | 2232068384 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
PageCount | 8 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_2232068384 pubmed_primary_31107412 crossref_primary_10_1097_j_pain_0000000000001618 crossref_citationtrail_10_1097_j_pain_0000000000001618 wolterskluwer_health_00006396-201910000-00013 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2019-October-01 2019-10-00 20191001 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2019-10-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 10 year: 2019 text: 2019-October-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Pain (Amsterdam) |
PublicationTitleAlternate | Pain |
PublicationYear | 2019 |
Publisher | Wolters Kluwer |
Publisher_xml | – name: Wolters Kluwer |
References | Vase (R78-20230818) 2015; 97 Handley (R44-20230818) 2002; 263 Brown (R11-20230818) 2013; 35 Hahn (R42-20230818) 1997; 26 Webster (R81-20230818) 2016; 35 Benedetti (R6-20230818) 1997; 71 Aslaksen (R3-20230818) 2015; 156 Edwards (R23-20230818) 2005; 10 Westermann (R83-20230818) 1996; 26 Campbell (R12-20230818) 2005; 113 Barsky (R5-20230818) 2002; 287 Caplandies (R13-20230818) 2017; 4 Benedetti (R7-20230818) 2007; 147 Bingel (R9-20230818) 2011; 3 Graham (R41-20230818) 2009; 60 Bijur (R8-20230818) 2003; 10 Freeman (R35-20230818) 2015; 15 Geuter (R40-20230818) 2013; 33 Collins (R15-20230818) 2001; 6 Vase (R79-20230818) 2003; 105 Klinger (R48-20230818) 2017; 2 Rose (R72-20230818) 2012; 35 Nouwen (R66-20230818) 2006; 7 Colloca (R20-20230818) 2008; 136 Mahr (R55-20230818) 2017; 26 Wilhelm (R84-20230818) 2018; 25 Halberstadt (R43-20230818) 1997; 72 Neumann (R62-20230818) 2001; 15 Fowler (R32-20230818) 2011; 34 Solomon (R77-20230818) 1974; 81 Petersen (R69-20230818) 2014; 155 Colloca (R19-20230818) 2011; 73 Baron (R4-20230818) 1976; 6 Schmid (R75-20230818) 2013; 154 Forgas (R30-20230818) 2008; 445 Fox (R33-20230818) 2016; 39 Leong (R52-20230818) 2015; 16 Leventhal (R53-20230818) 1979; 37 Wegener (R82-20230818) 1994; 66 Newman (R63-20230818) 2014; 17 Eccleston (R21-20230818) 1999; 125 Kennedy (R47-20230818) 1961; 95 Kong (R49-20230818) 2008; 28 Williamson (R85-20230818) 2005; 14 McCaul (R57-20230818) 1982; 43 Edwards (R22-20230818) 2017; 158 Colloca (R18-20230818) 2007; 20 Geers (R37-20230818) 2015; 38 Leknes (R51-20230818) 2008; 9 Niedenthal (R65-20230818) 2000; 30 Geers (R38-20230818) 2013; 105 Myers (R61-20230818) 2011; 5 O'Connor (R67-20230818) 1996; 49 Roderigo (R71-20230818) 2017; 158 Geers (R39-20230818) 2010; 11 Lee (R50-20230818) 2010; 11 Colagiuri (R14-20230818) 2018; 19 Ferrer (R28-20230818) 2015; 15 Pe (R68-20230818) 2012; 12 Faul (R27-20230818) 2007; 39 Montgomery (R60-20230818) 1997; 72 Winterich (R86-20230818) 2010; 36 Elsenbruch (R24-20230818) 2012; 24 Handley (R45-20230818) 2004; 40 Branscombe (R10-20230818) 1985; 9 Evers (R25-20230818) 2018; 87 Vögtle (R80-20230818) 2013; 154 Aslaksen (R2-20230818) 2015; 8 Niendthal (R64-20230818) 1999; 106 Scott (R76-20230818) 2008; 65 Lyby (R54-20230818) 2012; 153 Flaten (R29-20230818) 2011; 366 Montgomery (R59-20230818) 1996; 7 Fredrickson (R34-20230818) 1998; 12 Colloca (R17-20230818) 2017; 358 Kállai (R46-20230818) 2004; 112 Price (R70-20230818) 1999; 83 Ahles (R1-20230818) 1983; 7 Forsythe (R31-20230818) 2011; 12 Salovey (R73-20230818) 1989; 57 Schafer (R74-20230818) 2002; 7 Faasse (R26-20230818) 2015; 34 |
References_xml | – volume: 11 start-page: 681 year: 2010 ident: R50-20230818 article-title: Lower-order pain-related constructs are more predictive of cold pressor pain ratings than higher-order personality traits publication-title: J Pain doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.10.013 – volume: 6 start-page: 260 year: 1976 ident: R4-20230818 article-title: The reduction of human aggression: a field study of the influence of incompatible reactions publication-title: J Appl Soc Psychol doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1976.tb01330.x – volume: 6 start-page: 330 year: 2001 ident: R15-20230818 article-title: A comparison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in modern missing data procedures publication-title: Psychol Methods doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.6.4.330 – volume: 5 start-page: 297 year: 2011 ident: R61-20230818 article-title: Goodbye, listwise deletion: presenting hot deck imputation as an easy and effective tool for handling missing data publication-title: Commun Methods Measur doi: 10.1080/19312458.2011.624490 – volume: 136 start-page: 211 year: 2008 ident: R20-20230818 article-title: The role of learning in nocebo and placebo effects publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.02.006 – volume: 57 start-page: 539 year: 1989 ident: R73-20230818 article-title: Influence of mood on health-relevant cognitions publication-title: J Pers Soc Psychol doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.539 – volume: 125 start-page: 356 year: 1999 ident: R21-20230818 article-title: Pain demands attention: a cognitive–affective model of the interruptive function of pain publication-title: Psychol Bull doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.356 – volume: 7 start-page: 147 year: 2002 ident: R74-20230818 article-title: Missing data: our view of the state of the art publication-title: Psychol Methods doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147 – volume: 10 start-page: 390 year: 2003 ident: R8-20230818 article-title: Validation of a verbally administered numerical rating scale of acute pain for use in the emergency department publication-title: Acad Emerg Med doi: 10.1197/aemj.10.4.390 – volume: 12 start-page: 191 year: 1998 ident: R34-20230818 article-title: Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions publication-title: Cogn Emot doi: 10.1080/026999398379718 – volume: 9 start-page: 314 year: 2008 ident: R51-20230818 article-title: A common neurobiology for pain and pleasure publication-title: Nat Rev Neurosci doi: 10.1038/nrn2333 – volume: 97 start-page: 143 year: 2015 ident: R78-20230818 article-title: Nocebo vs placebo: the challenges of trial design in analgesia research publication-title: Clin Pharmacol Ther doi: 10.1002/cpt.31 – volume: 10 start-page: 219 year: 2005 ident: R23-20230818 article-title: Styles of pain coping predict cardiovascular function following a cold pressor test publication-title: Pain Res Manag doi: 10.1155/2005/216481 – volume: 87 start-page: 204 year: 2018 ident: R25-20230818 article-title: Implications of placebo and nocebo effects for clinical practice: consensus of a group of experts publication-title: Psychother Psychosomat doi: 10.1159/000490354 – volume: 106 start-page: 337 year: 1999 ident: R64-20230818 article-title: Emotional response categorization publication-title: Psychol Rev doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.2.337 – volume: 113 start-page: 20 year: 2005 ident: R12-20230818 article-title: Ethnic differences in responses to multiple experimental pain stimuli publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.08.013 – volume: 2 start-page: e586 year: 2017 ident: R48-20230818 article-title: Nocebo effects in clinical studies: hints for pain therapy publication-title: Pain Rep doi: 10.1097/PR9.0000000000000586 – volume: 34 start-page: 139 year: 2011 ident: R32-20230818 article-title: Concept priming and pain: an experimental approach to understanding the role of gender in sex-related pain differences publication-title: J Behav Med doi: 10.1007/s10865-010-9291-7 – volume: 158 start-page: 1489 year: 2017 ident: R71-20230818 article-title: Effects of acute psychological stress on placebo and nocebo responses in a clinically relevant model of visceroception publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000940 – volume: 20 start-page: 435 year: 2007 ident: R18-20230818 article-title: Nocebo hyperalgesia: how anxiety is turned into pain publication-title: Curr Opin Anesthesiol doi: 10.1097/ACO.0b013e3282b972fb – volume: 73 start-page: 598 year: 2011 ident: R19-20230818 article-title: The nocebo effect and its relevance for clinical practice publication-title: Psychosom Med doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182294a50 – volume: 36 start-page: 1467 year: 2010 ident: R86-20230818 article-title: Now that I'm sad, it's hard to be mad: the role of cognitive appraisals in emotional blunting publication-title: Pers Soc Psychol Bull doi: 10.1177/0146167210384710 – volume: 8 start-page: 703 year: 2015 ident: R2-20230818 article-title: Fear of pain potentiates nocebo hyperalgesia publication-title: J Pain Res doi: 10.2147/JPR.S91923 – volume: 445 start-page: 1362 year: 2008 ident: R30-20230818 article-title: On being happy and gullible: mood effects on skepticism and the detection of deception publication-title: J Exp Soc Psychol doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.010 – volume: 26 start-page: 731 year: 2017 ident: R55-20230818 article-title: Types, frequencies, and burden of nonspecific adverse events of drugs: analysis of randomized placebo-controlled clinical trials publication-title: Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf doi: 10.1002/pds.4169 – volume: 287 start-page: 622 year: 2002 ident: R5-20230818 article-title: Nonspecific medication side effects and the nocebo phenomenon publication-title: JAMA doi: 10.1001/jama.287.5.622 – volume: 263 start-page: 223 year: 2002 ident: R44-20230818 article-title: Mood and information processing: when happy and sad look the same publication-title: Motiv Emot doi: 10.1023/A:1021725130325 – volume: 83 start-page: 147 year: 1999 ident: R70-20230818 article-title: An analysis of factors that contribute to the magnitude of placebo analgesia in an experimental paradigm publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00081-0 – volume: 12 start-page: 1320 year: 2012 ident: R68-20230818 article-title: The dynamic interplay between emotions in daily life: augmentation, blunting, and the role of appraisal overlap publication-title: Emot doi: 10.1037/a0028262 – volume: 12 start-page: 563 year: 2011 ident: R31-20230818 article-title: Race and sex differences in primary appraisals, catastrophizing, and experimental pain outcomes publication-title: J Pain doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.11.003 – volume: 26 start-page: 557 year: 1996 ident: R83-20230818 article-title: Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: a meta-analysis publication-title: Eur J Soc Psychol doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199607)26:4<557::AID-EJSP769>3.0.CO;2-4 – volume: 154 start-page: 2372 year: 2013 ident: R75-20230818 article-title: Neural mechanisms mediating positive and negative treatment expectations in visceral pain: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study on placebo and nocebo effects in healthy volunteers publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.07.013 – volume: 153 start-page: 1114 year: 2012 ident: R54-20230818 article-title: Induced fear reduces the effectiveness of a placebo intervention on pain publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.02.042 – volume: 147 start-page: 260 year: 2007 ident: R7-20230818 article-title: When words are painful: unraveling the mechanisms of the nocebo effect publication-title: Neuroscience doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.02.020 – volume: 71 start-page: 135 year: 1997 ident: R6-20230818 article-title: Blockade of nocebo hyperalgesia by the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)03346-0 – volume: 17 start-page: 372 year: 2014 ident: R63-20230818 article-title: Missing data: five practical guidelines publication-title: Organ Res Methods doi: 10.1177/1094428114548590 – volume: 105 start-page: 17 year: 2003 ident: R79-20230818 article-title: The contribution of suggestion, desire, and expectation to placebo effects in irritable bowel syndrome patients. An empirical investigation publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00073-3 – volume: 66 start-page: 1034 year: 1994 ident: R82-20230818 article-title: Mood management across affective states: the hedonic contingency hypothesis publication-title: J Pers Soc Psychol doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.1034 – volume: 33 start-page: 13784 year: 2013 ident: R40-20230818 article-title: Facilitation of pain in the human spinal cord by nocebo treatment publication-title: J Neurosci doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2191-13.2013 – volume: 35 start-page: 462 year: 2012 ident: R72-20230818 article-title: Treatment choice and pain analgesia publication-title: J Behav Med doi: 10.1007/s10865-011-9374-0 – volume: 72 start-page: 1017 year: 1997 ident: R43-20230818 article-title: Emotional state and the use of stimulus dimensions in judgment publication-title: J Pers Soc Psych doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.5.1017 – volume: 3 start-page: 70ra14 year: 2011 ident: R9-20230818 article-title: The effect of treatment expectation on drug efficacy: imaging the analgesic benefit of the opioid remifentanil publication-title: Sci Transl Med doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3001244 – volume: 105 start-page: 549 year: 2013 ident: R38-20230818 article-title: Why does choice enhance treatment effectiveness? Using placebo treatments to demonstrate the role of personal control publication-title: J Pers Soc Psychol doi: 10.1037/a0034005 – volume: 60 start-page: 549 year: 2009 ident: R41-20230818 article-title: Missing data analysis: making it work in the real world publication-title: Annu Rev Psychol doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085530 – volume: 49 start-page: 1271 year: 1996 ident: R67-20230818 article-title: Framing effects on expectations, decisions, and side effects experienced: the case of influenza immunization publication-title: J Clin Epidemiol doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(96)00177-1 – volume: 25 start-page: 381 year: 2018 ident: R84-20230818 article-title: Decreasing the burden of side effects through positive message framing: an experimental proof-of-concept study publication-title: Int J Behav Med doi: 10.1007/s12529-018-9726-z – volume: 65 start-page: 220 year: 2008 ident: R76-20230818 article-title: Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergic responses publication-title: Arch Gen Psychiatr doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.34 – volume: 28 start-page: 13354 year: 2008 ident: R49-20230818 article-title: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study on the neural mechanisms of hyperalgesic nocebo effect publication-title: J Neurosci doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2944-08.2008 – volume: 7 start-page: 174 year: 1996 ident: R59-20230818 article-title: Mechanisms of placebo pain reduction: an empirical investigation publication-title: Psychol Sci doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00352.x – volume: 81 start-page: 119 year: 1974 ident: R77-20230818 article-title: An opponent–process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect publication-title: Psychol Rev doi: 10.1037/h0036128 – volume: 35 start-page: 436 year: 2013 ident: R11-20230818 article-title: Choice as a moderator of placebo expectation effects: additional support from two experiments publication-title: Basic Appl Soc Psych doi: 10.1080/01973533.2013.803968 – volume: 43 start-page: 154 year: 1982 ident: R57-20230818 article-title: Attention, distraction, and cold-pressor pain publication-title: J Pers Soc Psychol doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.43.1.154 – volume: 15 start-page: 725 year: 2001 ident: R62-20230818 article-title: The influence of mood on the intensity of emotional responses: disentangling feeling and knowing publication-title: Cogn Emot doi: 10.1080/02699930143000266 – volume: 39 start-page: 175 year: 2007 ident: R27-20230818 article-title: *Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences publication-title: Behav Res Methods doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 – volume: 35 start-page: 1334 year: 2016 ident: R81-20230818 article-title: A systematic review of factors that contribute to nocebo effects publication-title: Health Psychol doi: 10.1037/hea0000416 – volume: 19 start-page: 476 year: 2018 ident: R14-20230818 article-title: Autonomic arousal as a mechanism of the persistence of nocebo hyperalgesia publication-title: J Pain doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.12.006 – volume: 39 start-page: 527 year: 2016 ident: R33-20230818 article-title: Does choosing help everyone?: examining the relationships among treatment choice, self-construal, and pain publication-title: J Behav Med doi: 10.1007/s10865-015-9708-4 – volume: 11 start-page: 1165 year: 2010 ident: R39-20230818 article-title: Dispositional optimism predicts placebo analgesia publication-title: J Pain doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.02.014 – volume: 7 start-page: 159 year: 1983 ident: R1-20230818 article-title: Cognitive control of pain: attention to the sensory aspects of the cold pressor stimulus publication-title: Cognit Ther Res doi: 10.1007/BF01190070 – volume: 155 start-page: 1426 year: 2014 ident: R69-20230818 article-title: The magnitude of nocebo effects in pain: a meta-analysis publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.04.016 – volume: 40 start-page: 106 year: 2004 ident: R45-20230818 article-title: Affect and automatic mood maintenance publication-title: J Exp Soc Psychol doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00086-6 – volume: 37 start-page: 688 year: 1979 ident: R53-20230818 article-title: Effects of preparatory information about sensations, threat of pain, and attention on cold pressor distress publication-title: J Pers Soc Psychol doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.5.688 – volume: 24 start-page: 914 year: 2012 ident: R24-20230818 article-title: How positive and negative expectations shape the experience of visceral pain: an experimental pilot study in healthy women publication-title: Neurogastroenterol Motil doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2012.01950.x – volume: 38 start-page: 136 year: 2015 ident: R37-20230818 article-title: Prior experience with a pain stimulus predicts placebo analgesia publication-title: J Behav Med doi: 10.1007/s10865-014-9586-1 – volume: 7 start-page: 62 year: 2006 ident: R66-20230818 article-title: Effects of focusing and distraction on cold pressor–induced pain in chronic back pain patients and control subjects publication-title: J Pain doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.08.004 – volume: 156 start-page: 39 year: 2015 ident: R3-20230818 article-title: Opposite effects of the same drug: reversal of topical analgesia by nocebo information publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/j.pain.0000000000000004 – volume: 34 start-page: 880 year: 2015 ident: R26-20230818 article-title: Seeing is believing: impact of social modeling on placebo and nocebo responding publication-title: Health Psychol doi: 10.1037/hea0000199 – volume: 358 start-page: 44 year: 2017 ident: R17-20230818 article-title: Nocebo effects can make you feel pain publication-title: Science doi: 10.1126/science.aap8488 – volume: 26 start-page: 607 year: 1997 ident: R42-20230818 article-title: The nocebo phenomenon: concept, evidence and implications for public health publication-title: Prev Med doi: 10.1006/pmed.1996.0124 – volume: 154 start-page: 1427 year: 2013 ident: R80-20230818 article-title: Nocebo hyperalgesia induced by social observational learning publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.04.041 – volume: 4 start-page: 259 year: 2017 ident: R13-20230818 article-title: Effect type but not attribute framing alters nocebo headaches in an experimental paradigm publication-title: Psychol Conscious Theor Res Pract – volume: 72 start-page: 107 year: 1997 ident: R60-20230818 article-title: Classical conditioning and the placebo effect publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00016-X – volume: 30 start-page: 211 year: 2000 ident: R65-20230818 article-title: Emotional state and the detection of change in facial expression of emotion publication-title: Eur J Soc Psychol doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(200003/04)30:2<211::AID-EJSP988>3.0.CO;2-3 – volume: 15 start-page: 197 year: 2015 ident: R35-20230818 article-title: Distinct neural representations of placebo and nocebo effects publication-title: Neuroimage doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.015 – volume: 95 start-page: 203 year: 1961 ident: R47-20230818 article-title: The nocebo reaction publication-title: Med Exp Int J Exp Med – volume: 158 start-page: 846 year: 2017 ident: R22-20230818 article-title: Observer influences on pain: an experimental series examining same-sex and opposite-sex friends, strangers, and romantic partners publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000840 – volume: 9 start-page: 153 year: 1985 ident: R10-20230818 article-title: Effect of hedonic valence and physiological arousal on emotion: a comparison of two theoretical perspectives publication-title: Motiv Emot doi: 10.1007/BF00991573 – volume: 15 start-page: 732 year: 2015 ident: R28-20230818 article-title: Effectiveness of internet-based affect induction procedures: a systematic review and meta-analysis publication-title: Emotion doi: 10.1037/emo0000035 – volume: 112 start-page: 142 year: 2004 ident: R46-20230818 article-title: The effects of experimenter characteristics on pain reports in women and men publication-title: PAIN doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.08.008 – volume: 14 start-page: 798 year: 2005 ident: R85-20230818 article-title: Pain: a review of three commonly used pain rating scales publication-title: J Clin Nurs doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2005.01121.x – volume: 366 start-page: 1818 year: 2011 ident: R29-20230818 article-title: The relation of emotions to placebo responses publication-title: Philos Tras R Soc Lond B Biol Sci doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0407 – volume: 16 start-page: 1176 year: 2015 ident: R52-20230818 article-title: A perspective-taking manipulation leads to greater empathy and less pain during the cold pressor task publication-title: J Pain doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2015.08.006 |
SSID | ssj0002229 |
Score | 2.382384 |
Snippet | There is an ethical obligation to notify individuals about potential pain associated with diagnoses, treatments, and procedures; however, supplying this... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref wolterskluwer |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source Publisher |
StartPage | 2290 |
SubjectTerms | Adolescent Affect - physiology Cold Temperature - adverse effects Female Humans Hyperalgesia - diagnosis Hyperalgesia - etiology Hyperalgesia - psychology Male Nocebo Effect Pain Measurement - methods Pain Measurement - psychology Photic Stimulation - methods Random Allocation Self Report Suggestion Verbal Behavior - physiology Young Adult |
Title | Testing a positive-affect induction to reduce verbally induced nocebo hyperalgesia in an experimental pain paradigm |
URI | https://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&NEWS=n&CSC=Y&PAGE=fulltext&D=ovft&AN=00006396-201910000-00013 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/31107412 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2232068384 |
Volume | 160 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV3db9MwELdKJyHQhPhe-ZKReKtcEttJmseqWpnYGAha6FvkJM7W0TVVP5jgr-cudtKsG2LQh6iyEzvx_Xy-O5_vCHkj0zALu45iUmQ-k5kjWai5ZAFQWAvtS5UW3hbH_sFIvh9740aj7rW0XsWd5Ne150r-h6pQBnTFU7L_QNmqUSiA_0BfuAKF4XozGmOIDDxk2Da-Vz80UyYaMWjaJiwsypYLDM-q2_B5sZpOf5pK3PaHtSvO26egii4w3cdyotD8obbi_s9VEX91odLJyXldmP2EFSCh9s4x2kKK0KrMCu-0Tb5mPCbblY25P7W5HL-cqs2Wfl_N0cVyYpjWQF3MNubbr_mJcSWwrgG2LWuqcMPK6a3krt2AM983B4w7-pqykiWbHAMl9pw6h-UmvegV1m9CCp91cFBMVEr7w5QAm9Wu3OE__hgNRkdH0XB_PLxFdngAoleT7PQOP387rJZyTHaOGnv5jqWDYBi8_UNHl8WbKzrLXbJ7kaMbxPJ7cQqiJssM75N7VgmhPYOoB6ShZw_J7Q_WzeIRWVpgUUW3gEUrYNFVTg2waAksaoFFDbBoHVhQR9WM1oFF8dNoCazHZDTYH_YPmM3OwRLhdQWLPVdzD9i9K0DIdrmSOnXC1M-4SMI0ExmP3VQmLtyiQSiFYqF0FjpxGPDMUa54QpqzfKb3CE28TGgXho-nWqbaD1MFkmuWOFnsBVqHLeKXgxolNnQ9ZlCZRqULxVmErxxtU6NFnOrBuYne8vdHXpdUi4DT4vYZzIZ8vYxAkOaO3xVd2SJPDTmrRgWaUaTLW4Rdom9kTjMXvYAu4DOcFsWWGiu0r2c36O05ubOZTC9Ic7VY65cgCa_iVxavvwGbMrEE |
linkProvider | Library Specific Holdings |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Testing+a+positive-affect+induction+to+reduce+verbally+induced+nocebo+hyperalgesia+in+an+experimental+pain+paradigm&rft.jtitle=Pain+%28Amsterdam%29&rft.au=Geers%2C+Andrew+L&rft.au=Close%2C+Shane&rft.au=Caplandies%2C+Fawn+C&rft.au=Vogel%2C+Charles+L&rft.date=2019-10-01&rft.issn=1872-6623&rft.eissn=1872-6623&rft.volume=160&rft.issue=10&rft.spage=2290&rft_id=info:doi/10.1097%2Fj.pain.0000000000001618&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=0304-3959&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=0304-3959&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=0304-3959&client=summon |