Processing speed in perceptual visual crowding
In this study, a perceptual visual crowding paradigm was designed to quantitatively assess the detection speed of (un)crowded meaningful visual targets using eye-movement responses. This paradigm was tested in individuals with dyslexia and age-matched controls. Trials were shown on a monitor with an...
Saved in:
Published in | Journal of vision (Charlottesville, Va.) Vol. 19; no. 3; p. 9 |
---|---|
Main Authors | , , |
Format | Journal Article |
Language | English |
Published |
United States
01.03.2019
|
Subjects | |
Online Access | Get full text |
ISSN | 1534-7362 1534-7362 |
DOI | 10.1167/19.3.9 |
Cover
Abstract | In this study, a perceptual visual crowding paradigm was designed to quantitatively assess the detection speed of (un)crowded meaningful visual targets using eye-movement responses. This paradigm was tested in individuals with dyslexia and age-matched controls. Trials were shown on a monitor with an integrated eye tracker to 25 control and 11 dyslexic subjects without any known ocular problems. Each trial started with fixation of a central target. Next, four peripheral targets were shown (left, right, top, bottom), one being a duplicate of the central target. The duplicate was either surrounded by flankers (crowding trials) or shown in isolation (reference trials). The timing of the primary saccades were obtained as a measure for detection speed. The performance of the reference trials was significantly higher compared to the crowding trials (p < 0.05) and a 54% increase in saccadic reaction time (SRT) was found for the crowding trials. The linear mixed model revealed a significant effect of critical spacing and chart type. For the reference trials, no significant differences in SRT were found between dyslexic and control subjects. However, for the crowding trials, a significant increase of ∼13% in SRT was found in the dyslexic subjects. A first application of this paradigm showed that dyslexic subjects perform equally well in identifying visual targets in crowded as well as uncrowded scenes compared to controls. However, they seem to need more time to identify targets in crowded scenes, which might be related to the reading difficulties that they experience in general. |
---|---|
AbstractList | In this study, a perceptual visual crowding paradigm was designed to quantitatively assess the detection speed of (un)crowded meaningful visual targets using eye-movement responses. This paradigm was tested in individuals with dyslexia and age-matched controls. Trials were shown on a monitor with an integrated eye tracker to 25 control and 11 dyslexic subjects without any known ocular problems. Each trial started with fixation of a central target. Next, four peripheral targets were shown (left, right, top, bottom), one being a duplicate of the central target. The duplicate was either surrounded by flankers (crowding trials) or shown in isolation (reference trials). The timing of the primary saccades were obtained as a measure for detection speed. The performance of the reference trials was significantly higher compared to the crowding trials (p < 0.05) and a 54% increase in saccadic reaction time (SRT) was found for the crowding trials. The linear mixed model revealed a significant effect of critical spacing and chart type. For the reference trials, no significant differences in SRT were found between dyslexic and control subjects. However, for the crowding trials, a significant increase of ∼13% in SRT was found in the dyslexic subjects. A first application of this paradigm showed that dyslexic subjects perform equally well in identifying visual targets in crowded as well as uncrowded scenes compared to controls. However, they seem to need more time to identify targets in crowded scenes, which might be related to the reading difficulties that they experience in general. In this study, a perceptual visual crowding paradigm was designed to quantitatively assess the detection speed of (un)crowded meaningful visual targets using eye-movement responses. This paradigm was tested in individuals with dyslexia and age-matched controls. Trials were shown on a monitor with an integrated eye tracker to 25 control and 11 dyslexic subjects without any known ocular problems. Each trial started with fixation of a central target. Next, four peripheral targets were shown (left, right, top, bottom), one being a duplicate of the central target. The duplicate was either surrounded by flankers (crowding trials) or shown in isolation (reference trials). The timing of the primary saccades were obtained as a measure for detection speed. The performance of the reference trials was significantly higher compared to the crowding trials (p < 0.05) and a 54% increase in saccadic reaction time (SRT) was found for the crowding trials. The linear mixed model revealed a significant effect of critical spacing and chart type. For the reference trials, no significant differences in SRT were found between dyslexic and control subjects. However, for the crowding trials, a significant increase of ∼13% in SRT was found in the dyslexic subjects. A first application of this paradigm showed that dyslexic subjects perform equally well in identifying visual targets in crowded as well as uncrowded scenes compared to controls. However, they seem to need more time to identify targets in crowded scenes, which might be related to the reading difficulties that they experience in general.In this study, a perceptual visual crowding paradigm was designed to quantitatively assess the detection speed of (un)crowded meaningful visual targets using eye-movement responses. This paradigm was tested in individuals with dyslexia and age-matched controls. Trials were shown on a monitor with an integrated eye tracker to 25 control and 11 dyslexic subjects without any known ocular problems. Each trial started with fixation of a central target. Next, four peripheral targets were shown (left, right, top, bottom), one being a duplicate of the central target. The duplicate was either surrounded by flankers (crowding trials) or shown in isolation (reference trials). The timing of the primary saccades were obtained as a measure for detection speed. The performance of the reference trials was significantly higher compared to the crowding trials (p < 0.05) and a 54% increase in saccadic reaction time (SRT) was found for the crowding trials. The linear mixed model revealed a significant effect of critical spacing and chart type. For the reference trials, no significant differences in SRT were found between dyslexic and control subjects. However, for the crowding trials, a significant increase of ∼13% in SRT was found in the dyslexic subjects. A first application of this paradigm showed that dyslexic subjects perform equally well in identifying visual targets in crowded as well as uncrowded scenes compared to controls. However, they seem to need more time to identify targets in crowded scenes, which might be related to the reading difficulties that they experience in general. |
Author | Boer, Aleid C. van der Steen, Johannes Pel, Johan J. M. |
Author_xml | – sequence: 1 givenname: Johan J. M. surname: Pel fullname: Pel, Johan J. M. organization: Vestibular and Ocular Motor Research Group, Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, The Netherlands, j.pel@erasmusmc.nl – sequence: 2 givenname: Aleid C. surname: Boer fullname: Boer, Aleid C. organization: Vestibular and Ocular Motor Research Group, Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, The Netherlands – sequence: 3 givenname: Johannes surname: van der Steen fullname: van der Steen, Johannes organization: Vestibular and Ocular Motor Research Group, Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, The Netherlands |
BackLink | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30909294$$D View this record in MEDLINE/PubMed |
BookMark | eNpd0MtKxDAUBuAgI85FfQTpSlzYmkuTNksZvMGALnQd0uRUIp2mJq0yb2-HGUVm9Z8DHwf-M0eT1reA0DnBGSGiuCEyY5k8QjPCWZ4WTNDJv3mK5jF-YEwxx-QETRmWWFKZz1D2EryBGF37nsQOwCauTToIBrp-0E3y5eI2TPDfdjSn6LjWTYSzfS7Q2_3d6_IxXT0_PC1vV6lhhPQp1dTmFiqTU9CssFgIWec5ZZhYDUAFMdxSbbSAqtSCYwPjXnHAugQua7ZAV7u7XfCfA8RerV000DS6BT9ERYksylJSxkd6sadDtQaruuDWOmzUb8cRXO7AWCLGAPUfIVhtn6eIVEzJEV4fQON63Tvf9kG75pD_AJQHbr0 |
CitedBy_id | crossref_primary_10_3389_fnins_2024_1332701 crossref_primary_10_1167_tvst_13_9_8 crossref_primary_10_1016_j_visres_2020_03_001 |
Cites_doi | 10.1016/j.visres.2004.05.001 10.1016/0042-6989(73)90041-2 10.1167/7.2.24 10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.017 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00071-2 10.1016/S0275-5408(98)00056-8 10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.005 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00490 10.1016/j.visres.2012.11.008 10.1167/18.3.4 10.1038/s41598-018-27480-4 10.1167/4.12.12 10.1038/nn.2187 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.11.033 10.1016/j.visres.2007.03.005 10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.009 10.1167/17.1.33 10.1016/j.visres.2006.05.011 10.1038/226177a0 10.3109/09273972.2012.680232 10.3389/neuro.08.049.2009 10.1016/j.visres.2005.04.006 10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00072-8 10.1167/9.4.14 10.1007/s10339-012-0499-z 10.1167/16.5.18 10.1056/NEJM198705143162003 10.1167/7.2.20 10.1016/j.visres.2004.10.007 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00193-5 10.1167/15.1.8 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90227-A 10.1073/pnas.1205566109 10.1016/0028-3932(77)90116-6 10.1167/10.6.15 |
ContentType | Journal Article |
DBID | AAYXX CITATION CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 |
DOI | 10.1167/19.3.9 |
DatabaseName | CrossRef Medline MEDLINE MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE MEDLINE PubMed MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitle | CrossRef MEDLINE Medline Complete MEDLINE with Full Text PubMed MEDLINE (Ovid) MEDLINE - Academic |
DatabaseTitleList | MEDLINE MEDLINE - Academic |
Database_xml | – sequence: 1 dbid: NPM name: PubMed url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed sourceTypes: Index Database – sequence: 2 dbid: EIF name: MEDLINE url: https://proxy.k.utb.cz/login?url=https://www.webofscience.com/wos/medline/basic-search sourceTypes: Index Database |
DeliveryMethod | fulltext_linktorsrc |
Discipline | Medicine |
EISSN | 1534-7362 |
ExternalDocumentID | 30909294 10_1167_19_3_9 |
Genre | Journal Article |
GroupedDBID | --- 29L 2WC 53G 5GY 5VS AAFWJ AAYXX ABIVO ACGFO ADBBV AENEX AFPKN ALMA_UNASSIGNED_HOLDINGS BAWUL BCNDV CITATION CS3 DIK DU5 E3Z EBS EJD F5P FRP GROUPED_DOAJ GX1 KQ8 M~E OK1 OVT P2P RNS RPM TR2 TRV W2D W8F XSB CGR CUY CVF ECM EIF NPM 7X8 |
ID | FETCH-LOGICAL-c311t-2a2d4debc42ea37d0669f442301daee261c5d2aca6eb8a650ce5d2b5e0a8e59f3 |
ISSN | 1534-7362 |
IngestDate | Fri Jul 11 15:25:04 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 03 07:00:03 EDT 2025 Tue Jul 01 00:23:54 EDT 2025 Thu Apr 24 23:07:41 EDT 2025 |
IsDoiOpenAccess | false |
IsOpenAccess | true |
IsPeerReviewed | true |
IsScholarly | true |
Issue | 3 |
Language | English |
License | http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0 |
LinkModel | OpenURL |
MergedId | FETCHMERGED-LOGICAL-c311t-2a2d4debc42ea37d0669f442301daee261c5d2aca6eb8a650ce5d2b5e0a8e59f3 |
Notes | ObjectType-Article-1 SourceType-Scholarly Journals-1 ObjectType-Feature-2 content type line 23 |
OpenAccessLink | https://doi.org/10.1167/19.3.9 |
PMID | 30909294 |
PQID | 2197889235 |
PQPubID | 23479 |
ParticipantIDs | proquest_miscellaneous_2197889235 pubmed_primary_30909294 crossref_primary_10_1167_19_3_9 crossref_citationtrail_10_1167_19_3_9 |
ProviderPackageCode | CITATION AAYXX |
PublicationCentury | 2000 |
PublicationDate | 2019-03-01 |
PublicationDateYYYYMMDD | 2019-03-01 |
PublicationDate_xml | – month: 03 year: 2019 text: 2019-03-01 day: 01 |
PublicationDecade | 2010 |
PublicationPlace | United States |
PublicationPlace_xml | – name: United States |
PublicationTitle | Journal of vision (Charlottesville, Va.) |
PublicationTitleAlternate | J Vis |
PublicationYear | 2019 |
References | i1534-7362-19-3-9-Bouma3 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Chung3 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Siderov1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Chung2 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Chung1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Geiger1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Bouma1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Bouma2 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Levi2 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Shovman1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Levi1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Gori1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Lorusso1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Albonico1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Mele1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Whitney1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Cao1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Martelli1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-MartinezConde1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Bulakowski1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Hawelka1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Louie1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Zorzi1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Vlaskamp1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Simmers1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Coates1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Wallace1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Kooiker1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-VanGenderen1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Pelli3 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Pelli2 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Toet1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Kennedy1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Pelli1 i1534-7362-19-3-9-Hess1 |
References_xml | – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Lorusso1 doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.05.001 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Bouma2 doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(73)90041-2 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Louie1 doi: 10.1167/7.2.24 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Chung1 doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.017 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Chung2 doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00071-2 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Simmers1 doi: 10.1016/S0275-5408(98)00056-8 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Whitney1 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.005 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Cao1 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00490 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Siderov1 doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.11.008 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Albonico1 doi: 10.1167/18.3.4 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Coates1 doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27480-4 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Pelli1 doi: 10.1167/4.12.12 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Pelli2 doi: 10.1038/nn.2187 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-MartinezConde1 doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.11.033 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Levi1 doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.03.005 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Levi2 doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.009 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Wallace1 doi: 10.1167/17.1.33 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Shovman1 doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.05.011 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Bouma1 doi: 10.1038/226177a0 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-VanGenderen1 doi: 10.3109/09273972.2012.680232 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Bulakowski1 doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.049.2009 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Vlaskamp1 doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.04.006 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Chung3 doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00072-8 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Martelli1 doi: 10.1167/9.4.14 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Mele1 doi: 10.1007/s10339-012-0499-z – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Kooiker1 doi: 10.1167/16.5.18 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Geiger1 doi: 10.1056/NEJM198705143162003 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Pelli3 doi: 10.1167/7.2.20 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Hawelka1 doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2004.10.007 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Hess1 doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00193-5 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Gori1 doi: 10.1167/15.1.8 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Toet1 doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90227-A – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Zorzi1 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1205566109 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Bouma3 doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(77)90116-6 – ident: i1534-7362-19-3-9-Kennedy1 doi: 10.1167/10.6.15 |
SSID | ssj0020501 |
Score | 2.273611 |
Snippet | In this study, a perceptual visual crowding paradigm was designed to quantitatively assess the detection speed of (un)crowded meaningful visual targets using... |
SourceID | proquest pubmed crossref |
SourceType | Aggregation Database Index Database Enrichment Source |
StartPage | 9 |
SubjectTerms | Adult Cognition - physiology Crowding Dyslexia - physiopathology Eye Movements - physiology Female Fixation, Ocular - physiology Humans Male Motion Perception - physiology Perceptual Masking - physiology Pilot Projects Reaction Time - physiology Saccades - physiology Young Adult |
Title | Processing speed in perceptual visual crowding |
URI | https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30909294 https://www.proquest.com/docview/2197889235 |
Volume | 19 |
hasFullText | 1 |
inHoldings | 1 |
isFullTextHit | |
isPrint | |
link | http://utb.summon.serialssolutions.com/2.0.0/link/0/eLvHCXMwnV1ba9swFBZbB2Mvo7tn6zoPtkd7tiVb1mM7UkppusESyJuQ5BMWKHZZ3Q72sN--o4vtlGbQ7cVJhC0Sf87Rdy6fDiEfKIVaGaZjoSoVs5yZWOf4f1RGA-WVoiun456dlccLdrIsln2H7KAu6XRifm3VlfwPqjiGuFqV7D8gO0yKA_ge8cUjIozHO2EcqvxdSOAClyFfEu4qVawq5Hp9aV_Q1P4cVqjbPNSry10216be2w7J57VVCLoSWJVsxAq--pT-Sfvdip7GQOphG3pin8O6HuOuVhplt6r41kGQgNgLG7gRarDqpr7WarCOLOY0mE_YMtabVLHx6NAN-yi2W-3S6f5FQhMxrkt9Lv7sizxanJ7K-XQ5v08e5JxntnZz9ns6eNZpkYaNcf13CU2kcN5PftabrOMvroSjFPNd8jhgEB14YJ-Qe9A8JQ9nodrhGUlGfCOHb7RuohHfyOMb9fg-J4uj6fzzcRz6W8SGZlkX5yqvWQ3asBwU5TWyP7FiyG_TrFYA6Nuaos6VUSXoSiGVNoCfdQGpqqAQK_qC7DRtA69IxFmqQCC7W4mUQaordHML4FBypSvKswn52P9-acLm77YHybl0TmDJZSYklWJC3g3nXfjtTm6d8b6_fRItkU0vqQbaq0uJax-vKnQYigl56e_rMAdNRYpEnL2-w9VvyKPx4dsjO92PK3iLzK_T-y5isu-w_wOoTVo6 |
linkProvider | ISSN International Centre |
openUrl | ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ffmt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Processing+speed+in+perceptual+visual+crowding&rft.jtitle=Journal+of+vision+%28Charlottesville%2C+Va.%29&rft.au=Pel%2C+Johan+J+M&rft.au=Boer%2C+Aleid+C&rft.au=van+der+Steen%2C+Johannes&rft.date=2019-03-01&rft.issn=1534-7362&rft.eissn=1534-7362&rft.volume=19&rft.issue=3&rft.spage=9&rft_id=info:doi/10.1167%2F19.3.9&rft.externalDBID=NO_FULL_TEXT |
thumbnail_l | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/lc.gif&issn=1534-7362&client=summon |
thumbnail_m | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/mc.gif&issn=1534-7362&client=summon |
thumbnail_s | http://covers-cdn.summon.serialssolutions.com/index.aspx?isbn=/sc.gif&issn=1534-7362&client=summon |